Aichert, D. S., Wöstmann, N. M., Costa, A., Macare, C., Wenig, J. R., Möller, H. J., et al. (2012). Associations between trait impulsivity and prepotent response inhibition. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 1016–1032. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2012.706261
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ainsworth, B., Eddershaw, R., Meron, D., Baldwin, D. S., & Garner, M. (2013). The effect of focused attention and open monitoring meditation on attention network function in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 210, 1226–1231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.09.002.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Allen, M., Dietz, M., Blair, K. S., van Beek, M., Rees, G., Vestergaard-Poulsen, P., … Roepstorff, A. (2012). Cognitive-affective neural plasticity following active-controlled mindfulness intervention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(44), 15601–15610. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2957-12.2012.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Allom, V., Panetta, G., Mullan, B., & Hagger, M. (2016). Self-report and behavioural approaches to the measurement of self-control: Are we assessing the same construct? Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 137–142.
Article
Google Scholar
Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R. (2007). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 14, 449–463. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.544.
Article
Google Scholar
Andreu, C. L., Moenne-Loccoz, C., Lopez, V., Slagter, H. A., Franken, I. H. A., & Cosmelli, D. (2017). Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence of enhanced performance monitoring in meditators. Mindfulness, 8(6), 1603–1614. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0732-z.
Article
Google Scholar
Antón, E., Carreiras, M., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2019). The impact of bilingualism on executive functions and working memory in young adults. PLoS One, 14(2), e0206770.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Baijal, S., Jha, A. P., Kiyonaga, A., Singh, R., & Srinivasan, N. (2011). The influence of concentrative meditation training on the development of attention networks during early adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 153. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00153.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Banich, M. T., & Depue, B. E. (2015). Recent advances in understanding neural systems that support inhibitory control. Behavioral Sciences, 1, 17–22.
Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351–355.
Article
Google Scholar
Becerra, R., Dandrade, C., & Harms, C. (2017). Can specific attentional skills be modified with mindfulness training for novice practitioners? Current Psychology, 36, 657–664. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9454-y.
Article
Google Scholar
Benz, S., Sellaro, R., Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2016). Music makes the world go around: The impact of musical training on non-musical cognitive functions – A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2023.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122, 67–73.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & DePape, A.-M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 565–574. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012735.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Blom, E., Boerma, T., Bosma, E., Cornips, L., & Evaraert, E. (2017). Cognitive advantages of bilingual children in different sociolinguistic contexts. Frontiers, 8, 552. https://doi.org/10.3389/psyg.2017.00552.
Article
Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2014). Cognitive control in bilinguals: Advantages in stimulus-stimulus inhibition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 610–629.
Article
Google Scholar
Caine, M. S., Landau, A. N., & Shimamura, A. P. (2012). Action video game experience reduces the cost of switching tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(4), 641–647.
Article
Google Scholar
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130, 278–288.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97(3), 404–431.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Google Scholar
Chang, Y.-K., Pesce, C., Chiang, Y.-T., Kuo, C.-Y., & Fong, D.-Y. (2015). Antecedent acute cycling exercise affects attention control: an ERP study using attention network test. Frontiers, 9, 156. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00156.
Article
Google Scholar
Chen, Y., Spagna, A., Wu, T., Kim, T. H., Wu, Q., Chen, C., … Fan, J. (2019). Testing a cognitive model of human intelligence. Scientific Reports, 9, 2898. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-39685-2.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Christakou, A., Halari, R., Smith, A. B., Ifkovits, E., Brammer, M., & Rubia, K. (2009). Sex- dependent age modulation of frontostriatal and temporo-parietal activation during cognitive control. Neuroimage, 48, 223–236.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Chuderski, A., Taraday, M., Necka, E., & Smolen, T. (2012). Storage capacity explains fluid intelligence but executive control does not. Intelligence, 40, 278–295.
Article
Google Scholar
Colzato, L. S., Sellaro, R., Samara, I., & Hommel, B. (2015). Meditation-induced cognitive-control states regulate response-conflict adaptation: Evidence from trial-to-trial adjustments in the Simon task. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 110–114.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure “change” – or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029382.
Article
Google Scholar
D’Souza, A. A., Moradzadeh, L., & Wiseheart, M. (2018). Musical training, bilingualism, and executive function: working memory and inhibitory control. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3, 11, 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-018-0095-6.
Article
Google Scholar
de Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: a meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 76–99.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 34–48.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Donnelly, S., Brooks, P., & Homer, B. (2019). Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychological Bulletin & Review, 26(4), 1122–1147. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01567-z.
Article
Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(3), 259–268.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., & Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe: The organization of goal-directed behavior. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 257–303.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Dye, M. W. G., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2009). The development of attention skills in action video game players. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1780–1789. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.002.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12(10), 374–380.
Article
Google Scholar
Elliott, J. C., Wallace, B. A., & Giesbrecht, B. (2014). A week-long meditation retreat decouples behavioral measures of the alerting and executive attention networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 69. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00069.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, (vol. 44, pp. 145–199). New York: Elsevier.
Google Scholar
Enticott, P. G., Ogloff, J. R., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2006). Associations between laboratory measures of executive inhibitory control and self-reported impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 285–294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.01.011.
Article
Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J., & Hoffman, R. R. (2006). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(1), 143–149.
Article
Google Scholar
Esch, T., Winkler, J., Auwarter, V., Gnann, H., Huber, R., & Schmidt, S. (2017). Mindfulness in pain autoregulation: Unexpected results from a randomized-controlled trial and possible implications for meditation research. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 674. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00674.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Evans, K. L., & Hampson, E. (2015). Sex-dependent effects on tasks assessing reinforcement learning and interference inhibition. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1044.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fan, Y., Tang, Y.-Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2014). Short term integrative meditation improves resting alpha activity and Stroop performance. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 39, 213–217. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-014-9258-5.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Felver, J. C., Tipsord, J. M., Morris, M. J., Racer, K. H., & Dishion, T. J. (2017). The effects of mindfulness-based intervention on children’s attention regulation. Journal of Attention Disorder, 21(10), 872–881. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054714548032.
Article
Google Scholar
Field, A. (2018). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics, (5th ed., ). London: Sage Publications.
Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101–135. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101.
Article
Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2016). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186–204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.k2016.04.023.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17, 172–179.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 201–225.
Article
Google Scholar
Furley, P., & Wood, G. (2016). Working memory, attentional control, and expertise in sports: A review of current literature and directions for future research. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(4), 415–425.
Article
Google Scholar
Gobet, F., Johnston, S. J., Ferrufino, G., Johnson, M., Jones, M. B., Molyneux, A., … Weeden, L. (2014). Frontiers in Psychology, 28(5), 1337. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01337.
Article
Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Weissberger, G. H., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R. I., & Cera, C. M. (2011). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A multilingual naming test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 594–615.
Article
Google Scholar
Hillman, C. H., McAuley, E., Erickson, K. J., Liu-Ambrose, T., & Kramer, A. F. (2019). On mindful and mindless physical activity and executive function: A response to Diamond and Ling. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 37, 100529.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Homacka, S., Lee, D., & Ricco, C. A. (2005). Test review: Delis-Kaplan executive function system. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 599.
Article
Google Scholar
Hutchinson, C. V., Barrett, D. J. K., Nitka, A., & Raynes, K. (2016). Action video game training reduces the Simon Effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(2), 587–592. https://doi.org/10.3758/s1342.
Article
Google Scholar
Irons, J. L., Remington, R. W., & McLean, J. P. (2011). Not so fast: Rethinking the effects of action video games on attentional capacity. Australian Journal of Psychology, 2011(63), 224–231. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1742-9536.2011.00001.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Isbel, B., & Mahar, D. (2015). Cognitive mechanisms of mindfulness: A test of current models. Consciousness and Cognition, 38, 50–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.10.005.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Jauregi, A., Kessler, K., & Hassel, S. (2018). Linking cognitive measures of response inhibition and reward sensitivity to trait impulsivity. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2306. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02306.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119.
Article
Google Scholar
Jo, H.-G., Malinowski, P., & Schmidt, S. (2017). Frontal theta dynamics during response conflict in long-term mindfulness meditators. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 299. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00299.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kane, J. J., Conway, A. R., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. Variation in Working Memory, 1, 21–48.
Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., Meier, M. E., Smeekens, B. A., Gross, G. M., Chun, C. A., Silvia, P. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2016). Individual differences in the executive control of attention, memory, and thought, and their associations with schizotypy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 1017–1048.
Article
Google Scholar
Kornblum, S. (1994). The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli. Psychological Research, 56, 130–135.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lai, C., MacNeil, B., & Frewen, P. (2014). A comparison of the attentional effects of single-session meditation and Fp-HEG neurofeedback in novices. Mindfulness, 6(5), 1012–1020. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-014-0347-6.
Article
Google Scholar
Larson, M. J., Steffen, P. R., & Primosch, M. (2013). The impact of a brief mindfulness meditation intervention on cognitive control and error-related performance monitoring. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 308. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00308.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., de Bruin, A., & Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? Psychological Bulletin, 144, 394–425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lim, X., & Qu, L. (2016). The effect of single-session mindfulness training on preschool children’s attentional control. Mindfulness, 8(2), 300–310. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0600-2.
Article
Google Scholar
Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1994). Processing of an irrelevant location dimension as a function of the relevant stimulus dimension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(2), 286–298.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Irwing, P. (2004). Sex differences on the progressive matrices: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 32, 481–498.
Article
Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. M., Dodd, M. D., Sheard, E. D., Wilson, D. E., & Bibi, U. (2003). In opposition to inhibition. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, (vol. 43, pp. 163–214). New York: Elsevier Science.
Google Scholar
Magen, H., & Cohen, A. (2007). Modularity beyond perception: Evidence from single task interference paradigms. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 1–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017174.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Malesza, M., & Ostaszewski, P. (2016). Dark side of impulsivity— Associations between the Dark Triad, self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 88, 197–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.%2009.016.
Article
Google Scholar
Mason, L. A., Anders-Jefferson, R., Alvarado, K., Zimiga, B., Primero, L., Uvaydova, E., … Paap, K. R. (2018). Are autistic traits related to mental flexibility and self-control? In Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Association for Psychological Science, San Francisco.
Google Scholar
Mercier, J., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D. (2014). Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 89–117.
Article
Google Scholar
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75, 1373–1386.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 40–48.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Psychological Science, 21, 8–14.
Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Moore, A., Gruber, T., Derose, J., & Malinowski, P. (2012). Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00018.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 176–186.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., & Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1425–1433.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Norris, C. J., Creem, D., Hendler, R., & Kober, H. (2018). Brief mindfulness meditation improves attention in novices: Evidence from ERPs and moderation by neuroticism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 315. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00315.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Northey, J. M., Cherbuin, N., Pumpa, K. L., Smee, D. J., & Rattray, B. (2017). Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: A systematic review with meta-analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Oken, B., Wahbeh, H., Goodrich, E., Klee, D., Memmott, T., Miller, M., & Fu, R. (2016). Meditation in stressed older adults: Improvements in self-rate mental health not paralleled by improvements in cognitive function or physiological measures. Mindfulness, 8(3), 627–638. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0640-7.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Otten, S., Schotz, E., Wittmann, M., Kohls, N., Schmidt, S., & Meissner, K. (2015). Psychophysiology of duration estimation in experienced mindfulness meditators and matched controls. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1215. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01215.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R. (2018a). Bilingualism in cognitive science. In A. De Houwer, & L. Ortega (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism, (pp. 435–465). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R. (2018b). Bilingualism and executive functioning. In J. Altarriba, & R. Heredia (Eds.), An introduction to bilingualism: Principles and processes, (2nd ed., pp. 189–222). New York: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R. (2019). The bilingual advantage debate: Quantity and quality of the evidence. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The handbook of the neuroscience of multilingualism, (pp. 701–735). London: Wiley-Blackwell.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Anders-Jefferson, R., Mason, L., Alvarado, K., & Zimiga, B. (2018). Bilingual advantages in inhibition or attentional control: More challenges. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01409.
Paap, K. R., Anders-Jefferson, R., Mikulinsky, R., Masuda, S., & Mason, L. (2019). On the encapsulation of bilingual language control. Journal of Memory and Language, 105, 76–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.12.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 615–639.
Article
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2016). Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive functioning continue? Cortex, 74, 305–314.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Myuz, H. A., Anders, R. T., Bockelman, M. F., Mikulinsky, R., & Sawi, O. M. (2017). No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(2), 89–112.
Article
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 962.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. M. (2016). The role of test-retest reliability in measuring individual and group differences in executive functioning. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 274, 81–93.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768–774. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pettigrew, C., & Martin, R. C. (2014). Cognitive declines in healthy aging: Evidence from multiple aspects of interference resolution. Psychology and Aging, 29, 187–204. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036085.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Polak, E. L. (2009). Impact of two sessions of mindfulness training on attention, Unpublished doctoral dissertation (). Miami: University of Miami.
Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (2018). Rehabilitating the brain through meditation and electrical stimulation. Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.07.022.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Quan, P., Wang, W., Chu, C., & Zhou, L. (2018). Seven days of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy improves attention and coping style. Social Behavior and Personality, 46(3), 421–430. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.6623.
Article
Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1939). The RECI series of perceptual tests. An experimental survey. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 18, 16–34.
Article
Google Scholar
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1977). Manual for Raven’s advanced progressive matrices: Sets I and II. England: H.K. Lewis & Co. Ltd.
Google Scholar
Rey-Mermet, A., Gade, M., & Oberauer, K. (2018). Should we stop thinking about inhibition? Searching for individual and age differences in inhibition ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 501–526. https://doi.org/10.1037//xlm0000450.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rey-Mermet, A., Gade, M., Oberauer, K. (2019). Is executive control related to working memory capacity and fluid intelligence? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(8), 1335–1372. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000593.
Article
Google Scholar
Reynolds, B., Ortengren, A., Richards, J. B., & de Wit, H. (2006). Dimensions of impulsive behavior: personality and behavioral measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 305–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.024.
Article
Google Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Lalwani, L. N., & Velez-Uribe, I. (2015). The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000309.
Article
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). When the music’s over. Does music skill transfer to children’s and young adolescents’ cognitive and academic skills? A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 20, 55–67.
Article
Google Scholar
Sala, G., Tatlidil, K. S., & Gobet, F. (2017). Video game training does not enhance cognitive ability: A comprehensive meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 14, 111–139. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000139.
Article
Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (2010). Is flanker-based inhibition related to age? Identifying specific influences of individual differences on neurocognitive variables. Brain and Cognition, 73, 51–61.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 566–594.
Article
Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., Pink, J. E., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2008). Contextual analysis of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 36, 464–486.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Schotz, E., Otten, S., Wittmann, M., Schmidt, S., Kohls, N., & Meissner, K. (2015). Time perception, mindfulness and attentional capacities in transcendental meditators and matched controls. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 16–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.10.023.
Article
Google Scholar
Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027448.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Simon, J. R., & Small Jr., A. M. (1969). Processing auditory information: Interference from an irrelevant cue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 433–435. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028034.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Slevc, I. R., Davey, N. S., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. (2016). Tuning the mind: Exploring the connections between musical ability and executive functions. Cognition, 152, 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.017.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man: Their nature and measurement. London: Macmillan.
Google Scholar
Sperduti, M., Makowski, D., & Piolino, P. (2016). The protective role of long-term meditation on the decline of the executive component of attention in aging: a preliminary cross-sectional study. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 23(6), 691–702. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1159652.
Article
Google Scholar
Stahl, C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., Nuszbaum, M., Tüscher, O., Lieb, K., & Klauer, K. C. (2014). Behavioral components of impulsivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 850–886. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033981.
Article
Google Scholar
Stoet, G. (2016). Sex differences in the Simon task help to interpret sex differences in selective attention. Psychological Research, 81(3), 571–581.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Tang, Y. Y. (2009). Exploring the brain, optimizing the life. Beijing: Science Press (in Chinese).
Google Scholar
Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., et al. (2007). Short term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 17152–17156.
Article
Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–322.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Themanson, J. R., & Hillman, C. H. (2006). Cardiorespiratory fitness and acute aerobic exercise effects on neuroelectric and behavioral measures of action monitoring. Neuroscience, 141, 757–767. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.004.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Tibshirani, R. (2011). Regression shrinkage and selection via lasso: a retrospective. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 73(3), 273–282.
Article
Google Scholar
Toner, J., & Moran, A. (2014). In praise of conscious awareness: A new framework for the investigation of continuous improvement in expert athletes. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 769.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Unsworth, N., Redick, T. S., McMillan, B. D., Hambrick, D. Z., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2015). Is playing video games related to cognitive abilities? Psychological Science, 26(6), 759–774.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Unsworth, N., & Spillers, G. J. (2010). Working memory capacity: Attention control, secondary memory, or both? A direct test of the dual-component model. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 392–406.
Article
Google Scholar
Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2009). Psychology Science Quarterly, 51, 388–402.
Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24.
Article
Google Scholar
van den Hurk, P. A. M., Giommi, F., Gielen, S. C., Speckens, A. E. M., & Barendregt, H. P. (2010). Greater efficiency in attentional processing related to mindfulness meditation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(6), 1168–1180. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903249365.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
van den Hurk, P. A. M., van Aalderen, J. R., Giommi, F., Donders, R., Barendregt, H. P., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2012). An investigation of the role of attention in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for recurrently depressed patients. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3(1), 103–120. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.024811.
Article
Google Scholar
Vestberg, T., Gustafson, R., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., & Petrovic, P. (2012). Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players. PLoS One, 7(4), 1–5.
Article
Google Scholar
Wahbeh, H., Goodrich, E., Goy, E., & Oken, B. S. (2016). Mechanistic pathways of mindfulness meditation in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 72(4), 365–383. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22255.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1955). Wechsler adult intelligence scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Google Scholar
Wei, G.-X., Dong, H.-M., Yang, Z., Luo, J., & Zuo, X.-N. (2015). Tai Chi Chuan optimizes the functional organization of the intrinsic human brain architecture in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6, 74. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00074.
Article
Google Scholar
Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Alternative Therapies, 11(2), 42–58.
Google Scholar
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669–689.
Article
Google Scholar
Wilbertz, T., Deserno, L., Horstmann, A., Neumann, J., Villringer, A., Heinze, H. J., et al. (2014). Response inhibition and its relation to multidimensional impulsivity. Neuroimage, 103, 241–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.%2009.021.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wittmann, M., Otten, S., Schotz, E., Sarikaya, A., Lehnen, H., Jo, H.-G., … Meissner, K. (2015). Subjective expansion of extended time-spans in experienced meditators. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1586. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01586.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wolff, M., Kronke, K.-M., Venz, J., Kräplin, A., Bühringer, G., Smolka, M. N., & Goschke, T. (2016). Action versus state orientation moderates the impact of executive functioning on real-life self-control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(12), 1635–1653.
Article
Google Scholar
Yarkoni, T., & Westfall, J. (2017). Choosing prediction over explanation in psychology: Lessons from machine learning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 1100–1122. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617693393.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar