Abutalebi, J., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., & Weekes, B. S. (2015). The neuroprotective effects of bilingualism upon the inferior parietal lobule: A structural neuroimaging study in aging Chinese bilinguals. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 33, 3–13 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2014.09.008.
Article
Google Scholar
Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80, 207–245 https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310368803.
Article
Google Scholar
Amer, T., Kalender, B., Hasher, L., Trehub, S. E., & Wong, Y. (2013). Do older professional musicians have cognitive advantages. PLoS One, 8, e71630 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071630.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., Davidson, D. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 398 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Asaridou, S. S., & McQueen, J. M. (2013). Speech and music shape the listening brain: Evidence for shared domain-general mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 321 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00321.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839 https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47–89 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1.
Article
Google Scholar
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 699–714 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.003.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Besson, M., Chobert, J., & Marie, C. (2011). Transfer of training between music and speech: Common processing, attention, and memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 94 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00094.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn?: A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637 https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.612.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(1), 3–11 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003477.
Article
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2011). Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 229–235 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025406.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(4), 859–873 https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303 https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240–250 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & DePape, A.-M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 565–574 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012735.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language, 109(2), 93–100 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.001.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Hawrylewicz, Wiseheart, M., & Toplak, M. (2017). Interaction of bilingualism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(3), 588–601 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000887.
Article
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(1), 103–119 https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069050090010701.
Article
Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 696–705 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037254.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bidelman, G. M., Hutka, S., & Moreno, S. (2013). Tone language speakers and musicians share enhanced perceptual and cognitive abilities for musical pitch: Evidence for bidirectionality between the domains of language and music. PLoS One, 8(4), e60676 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060676.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Blackwell, K. A., Chatham, C. H., Wiseheart, M., & Munakata, Y. (2014). A developmental window into trade-offs in executive function: The case of task switching versus response inhibition in 6-year-olds. Neuropsychologia, 62, 356–364 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.04.016.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14(2), 256–269 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00974.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bugos, J. A., Perlstein, W. M., McCrae, C. S., Brophy, T. S., & Bedenbaugh, P. H. (2007). Individualized piano instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 11(4), 464–471 https://doi.org/10.1080/13607860601086504.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33(2), 301–311 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00583-9.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278–288 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Carey, D., Rosen, S., Krishnan, S., Pearce, M. T., Shepherd, A., Aydelott, J., & Dick, F. (2015). Generality and specificity in the effects of musical expertise on perception and cognition. Cognition, 137, 81–105 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.005.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cepeda, N. J., Blackwell, K. A., & Munakata, Y. (2013). Speed isn’t everything: Complex processing speed measures mask individual differences and developmental changes in executive control. Developmental Science, 16(2), 269–286 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12024.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavior sciences, (2nd ed., ). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Google Scholar
Cooper, A., & Wang, Y. (2012). The influence of linguistic and musical experience on Cantonese word learning. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131(6), 4756–4769 https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4714355.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Corrigall, K. A., Schellenberg, E. G., & Misura, N. M. (2013). Music training, cognition, and personality. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 222 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00222.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135–149 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Costa-Giomi, E. (1999). The effects of three years of piano instruction on children's cognitive development. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47(3), 198–212 https://doi.org/10.2307/3345779.
Article
Google Scholar
Cribbie, R. A. (2017). Multiplicity control, school uniforms, and other perplexing debates. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 49(3), 159–165 https://doi.org/10.1037/cbs0000075.
Article
Google Scholar
De Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26(1), 99–107.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Degé, F., Kubicek, C., & Schwarzer, G. (2011). Music lessons and intelligence: A relation mediated by executive functions. Music Perception, 29(2), 195–201 https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.195.
Article
Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61, 234–251 https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000243.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149 https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267.
Article
Google Scholar
Faroqi-Shah, Y., Sampson, M., Pranger, M., & Baughman, S. (2016). Cognitive control, word retrieval and bilingual aphasia: Is there a relationship? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 45, 95–109 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.07.001.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160 https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Feng, X., Diamond, A., & Bialystok, E. (2007). Manipulating information in working memory: An advantage for bilinguals. Poster session presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in child development, Boston, MA.
Fiveash, A., & Pammer, K. (2014). Music and language: Do they draw on similar syntactic working memory resources? Psychology of Music, 42(2), 190–209.
Article
Google Scholar
Franklin, M. S., Moore, K. S., Yip, C.-Y., Jonides, J., Rattray, K., & Moher, J. (2008). The effects of musical training on verbal memory. Psychology of Music, 36(3), 353–365 https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607086044.
Article
Google Scholar
Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Ross, B., Kakigi, R., & Pantev, C. (2004). Musical Training Enhances Automatic Encoding of Melodic Contour and Interval Structure. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(6), 1010–1021 https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929041502706.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fujioka, T., Ross, B., Kakigi, R., Pantev, C., & Trainor, L. J. (2006). One year of musical training affects development of auditory cortical-evoked fields in young children. Brain, 129(10), 2593–2608 https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl247.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
George, E. M., & Coch, D. (2011). Music training and working memory: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1083–1094 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.001.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 1–15 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728903001007.
Article
Google Scholar
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(2), 387–396 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3837-12.2013.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Hanna-Pladdy, B., & Gajewski, B. (2012). Recent and past musical activity predicts cognitive aging variability: Direct comparison with general lifestyle activities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 198 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00198.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Hanna-Pladdy, B., & MacKay, A. (2011). The relation between instrumental musical activity and cognitive aging. Neuropsychology, 25(3), 378–386 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021895.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Hannon, E. E., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Music acquisition: Effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 466–472.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hansen, M., Wallentin, M., & Vuust, P. (2013). Working memory and musical competence of musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of Music, 41(6), 779–793 https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735612452186.
Article
Google Scholar
Hurwitz, I., Wolff, P. H., Bortnick, B. D., & Kokas, K. (1975). Nonmusicol effects of the kodaly music curriculum in primary grade children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 8(3), 167–174 https://doi.org/10.1177/002221947500800310.
Article
Google Scholar
Hutka, S., Bidelman, G. M., & Moreno, S. (2013). Brain signal variability as a window into the bidirectionality between music and language processing: Moving from a linear to a nonlinear model. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 984 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00984.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Johansson, B. B. (2008). Language and music: What do they have in common and how do they differ? A neuroscientific approach. European Review, 16(04), 413–427.
Article
Google Scholar
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test, (2nd ed., ). Bloomington: Pearson, Inc.
Google Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012a). Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 356–369 https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.604788.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012b). Conflict monitoring and resolution: Are two languages better than one? Evidence from reaction time and event-related brain potentials. Brain Research, 1446, 71–90 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.01.052.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lee, Y., Lu, M., & Ko, H. (2007). Effects of skill training on working memory capacity. Learning and Instruction, 17(3), 336–344 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.02.010.
Article
Google Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621 https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.795574.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Luo, L., Craik, F. I., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 28–34 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030875.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mehr, S. A., Schachner, A., Katz, R. C., & Spelke, E. S. (2013). Two randomized trials provide no consistent evidence for nonmusical cognitive benefits of brief preschool music enrichment. PLoS One, 8(12), e82007 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082007.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Meinz, E. J., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). Deliberate practice is necessary but not sufficient to explain individual differences in piano sight-reading skill the role of working memory capacity. Psychological Science, 21(7), 914–919 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610373933.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Meuter, R. F., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 25–40 https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2602.
Article
Google Scholar
Mindt, M. R., Arentoft, A., Germano, K. K., D’Aquila, E., Scheiner, D., Pizzirusso, M., … Gollan, T. H. (2008). Neuropsychological, cognitive, and theoretical considerations for evaluation of bilingual individuals. Neuropsychology Review, 18(3), 255–268 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-008-9069-7.
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100 https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mok, P. P., & Zuo, D. (2012). The separation between music and speech: Evidence from the perception of Cantonese tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(4), 2711–2720.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Moradzadeh, L., Blumenthal, G., & Wiseheart, M. (2014). Musical training, bilingualism, and executive function: A closer look at task switching and dual-task performance. Cognitive Science, 39(5), 992–1020 https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12183.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Moreno, S. (2009). Can music influence language and cognition? Contemporary Music Review, 28(3), 329–345 https://doi.org/10.1080/07494460903404410.
Article
Google Scholar
Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., & Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1425–1433 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611416999.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Moreno, S., Wodniecka, Z., Tays, W., Alain, C., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Inhibitory control in bilinguals and musicians: Event related potential (ERP) evidence for experience-specific effects. PLoS One, 9(4), e94169 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094169.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519–533 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9.
Article
Google Scholar
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10(6), 719–726 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Nutley, S. B., Darki, F., & Klingberg, T. (2014). Music practice is associated with development of working memory during childhood and adolescence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 926 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00926.
Google Scholar
Oechslin, M. S., Van De Ville, D., Lazeyras, F., Hauert, C.-A., & James, C. E. (2013). Degree of musical expertise modulates higher order brain functioning. Cerebral Cortex, 23(9), 2213–2224 https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs206.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Owen, S. V., & Froman, R. D. (1998). Focus on qualitative methods uses and abuses of the analysis of covariance. Research in Nursing and Health, 21(6), 557–562 https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-240X(199812)21:6<557::AID-NUR9>3.0.CO;2-Z.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Paap, K., Wagner, S., Johnson, H., Bockelman, M., Cushing, D., & Sawi, O. (2014). 20,000 flanker task trials: Are the effects stable reliable, robust, and stable? Poster session presented at the Association for Psychological Science meeting. San Francisco, CA.
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 615–639 https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.944914.
Article
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 962 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., & Kraus, N. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear and Hearing, 30(6), 653–661 https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181b412e9.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Patel, A. D. (2011). Why would musical training benefit the neural encoding of speech? The OPERA hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 142 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00142.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Patel, A. D. (2014). Can nonlinguistic musical training change the way the brain processes speech? The expanded OPERA hypothesis. Hearing Research, 308, 98–108 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2013.08.011.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pelham, S. D., & Abrams, L. (2014). Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 313–325 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035224.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Peretz, I. (2006). The nature of music from a biological perspective. Cognition, 100(1), 1–32 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.004.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Prior, A. (2012). Too much of a good thing: Stronger bilingual inhibition leads to larger lag-2 task repetition costs. Cognition, 125(1), 1–12 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.019.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ratiu, I., & Azuma, T. (2015). Working memory capacity: Is there a bilingual advantage? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(1), 1–11 https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.976226.
Article
Google Scholar
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Band, G. P., & Logan, G. D. (1999). A study of adaptive behavior: Effects of age and irrelevant information on the ability to inhibit one’s actions. Acta Psychologica, 101(2), 315–337 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00010-4.
Article
Google Scholar
Roden, I., Kreutz, G., & Bongard, S. (2012). Effects of a school-based instrumental music program on verbal and visual memory in primary school children. A longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 572 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00572.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Rogalsky, C., Rong, F., Saberi, K., & Hickok, G. (2011). Functional anatomy of language and music perception: Temporal and structural factors investigated using functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(10), 3843–3852 https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4515-10.2011.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Sabers, D. L., & Franklin Jr., M. R. (1986). Increased power through design and analysis. The Journal of Experimental Education, 54(2), 101–104 https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1986.10806405.
Article
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Does far transfer exist? Negative evidence from chess, music, and working memory training. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 515–520 https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417712760.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Salvatierra, J. L., & Rosselli, M. (2010). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 26–37 https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910371021.
Article
Google Scholar
Schellenberg, E. G. (2011). Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 283–302 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02000.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Schellenberg, E. G. (2016). Music and nonmusical abilities. In G. E. McPherson (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development, (2nd ed., pp. 149–176). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198744443.001.0001.
Google Scholar
Seinfeld, S., Figueroa, H., Ortiz-Gil, J., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2013). Effects of music learning and piano practice on cognitive function, mood and quality of life in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(810) https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00810.
Slevc, L. R. (2012). Language and music: Sound, structure, and meaning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(4), 483–492 https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1186.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Slevc, L. R., Davey, N. S., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2016). Tuning the mind: Exploring the connections between musical ability and executive functions. Cognition, 152, 199–211 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.017.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Slevc, L. R., & Okada, B. M. (2015). Processing structure in language and music: A case for shared reliance on cognitive control. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(3), 637–652 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0712-4.
Article
Google Scholar
Strait, D. L., Kraus, N., Parbery-Clark, A., & Ashley, R. (2010). Musical experience shapes top-down auditory mechanisms: Evidence from masking and auditory attention performance. Hearing Research, 261(1), 22–29 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2009.12.021.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662 https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651.
Article
Google Scholar
Swaminathan, S., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2014). Arts education, academic achievement and cognitive ability. In P. L. Tinio, & J. K. Smith (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of the psychology of aesthetics and the arts, (pp. 364–384). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Travis, F., Harung, H. S., & Lagrosen, Y. (2011). Moral development, executive functioning, peak experiences and brain patterns in professional and amateur classical musicians: Interpreted in light of a unified theory of performance. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1256–1264 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.020.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2006). Simple and complex memory spans and their relation to fluid abilities: Evidence from list-length effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(1), 68–80 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.06.003.
Article
Google Scholar
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37(3), 498–505 https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192720.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000522.
Article
Google Scholar
Vega-Mendoza, M., West, H., Sorace, A., & Bak, T. H. (2015). The impact of late, non-balanced bilingualism on cognitive performance. Cognition, 137, 40–46 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.008.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Von Bastian, C. C., Souza, A. S., & Gade, M. (2016). No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(2), 246 https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000120.
Article
Google Scholar
Wallentin, M., Nielsen, A. H., Friis-Olivarius, M., Vuust, C., & Vuust, P. (2010). The musical ear test, a new reliable test for measuring musical competence. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(3), 188–196 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.02.004.
Article
Google Scholar
Wan, C. Y., & Schlaug, G. (2010). Music making as a tool for promoting brain plasticity across the life span. The Neuroscientist, 16, 566–577 https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858410377805.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scales–revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Google Scholar
Wiseheart, M., Viswanathan, M., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 141–146 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000273.
Article
Google Scholar
Zinbarg, R. E., Suzuki, S., Uliaszek, A. A., & Lewis, A. R. (2010). Biased parameter estimates and inflated type I error rates in analysis of covariance (and analysis of partial variance) arising from unreliability: Alternatives and remedial strategies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 307–319 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017552.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Zuk, J., Benjamin, C., Kenyon, A., & Gaab, N. (2014). Behavioral and neural correlates of executive functioning in musicians and non-musicians. PLoS One, 9(6), e99868 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099868.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar