Skip to main content
  • Review article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Cognitive perspectives on maintaining physicians’ medical expertise: V. Using a motivational framework to understand the benefits and costs of testing

Abstract

We apply a motivational perspective to understand the implications of physicians’ longitudinal assessment. We review the literature on situated expectancy-value theory, achievement goals, mindsets, anxiety, and stereotype threat in relation to testing and assessment. This review suggests several motivational benefits of testing as well as some potential challenges and costs posed by high-stakes, standardized tests. Many of the motivational benefits for testing can be understood from the equation of having the perceived benefits of the test outweigh the perceived costs of preparing for and taking the assessment. Attention to instructional framing, test purposes and values, and longitudinal assessment frameworks provide vehicles to further enhance motivational benefits and reduce potential costs of assessment.

Significance

Physicians in the USA are required to take continuing education assessments at various points throughout their careers. The medical boards that administer those assessments are considering changes in their structure and implementation, including a more longitudinal assessment model. Understanding the role that motivation can play for learners in both preparing for and taking continued education assessments can inform the assessments’ design, purpose, and the policies for giving them. We take a motivational perspective on the potential benefits and costs of testing and the implications of longitudinal assessment. We review prior research on motivation for learning from cognitive, social, and educational psychology, including studies from both laboratory and classroom settings. This analysis reveals that perceived test difficulty and expectations of success, instruction framing and feedback, alignment to the values of the learner, and creating multiple lower-stakes assessment opportunities are critical issues to consider when redesigning and implementing continued educational assessments to enhance motivation, learning, and performance.

Introduction

Physicians in the USA are required to take continuing education assessments at various points throughout their careers. Currently, many of these assessments take place every 5 to 10 years and can be viewed as summative assessments; however, many specialty boards are considering transitioning to shorter, more longitudinal assessments that can also serve as learning opportunities. This change presents an opportunity to consider some of the factors that could enhance the learning value of these assessments or otherwise make them more motivating for physicians.

In particular, for physicians to be motivated to participate in longitudinal assessments and other learning opportunities, they must view participation as having relatively high value and low costs. One general approach that can help us understand the role of value and costs in learning is the expectancy-value theory from social and educational psychology (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002, 2020; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000; Wigfield et al., 2016). This theory is widely used to explain, understand, and predict human motivation in learning and in academic performance. Expectancy-value theory posits that learners’ pursuit of an educational goal (i.e., their motivation to learn) is a function of the perceived benefits of pursuing the goal, the perceived costs of pursuing it, and the chance of succeeding if they do pursue the goal (their expectancies), as seen in Eq. 1 as follows:

$${\text{Motivation}}\;{\text{to}}\;{\text{Learn}} = {\text{Expectancies}}*\left( {{\text{Benefits}} - {\text{Cost}}} \right)$$
(1)

Thus, all other things being equal, physicians—and other learners—should be more motivated to study and practice their skills when there is a clear benefit for doing so (e.g., new knowledge, feedback on knowledge and skills, continued certification), when the costs of doing so are relatively minor (e.g., reasonable time and effort required), and when there is an expectation of success.

In this paper—part of a collection of five articles in this special issue focused on how physicians maintain medical expertise across their careers—we describe a motivational framework that builds on the expectancy-value theory that also connects to several related motivational theories and ideas from research on achievement goals, mindsets, stereotype threat, and test anxiety. We adopt the approach of a narrative review, not systematic, because we cover a wide variety of topics. To situate the strength of the evidence and claims made, we attach evidence levels (EL) to in-text citations for empirical claims (see Table 1). Evidence levels range from 1 to 6, with 1 being the strongest evidence (meta-analyses) and 6 being the weakest (opinion papers).

Table 1 Evidence levels for in-text citations for empirical claims

Understanding motivation with situated expectancy-value theory

The most recent articulation of the expectancy-value theory has been called the situated expectancy-value theory (Eccles & Wigfield, 2020). This version of the theory accounts for both long-term trajectories in the development of expectancies and values as well as the short-term psychological processes engaged in task choice and performance. This version of the theory strongly emphasizes the situated nature of the factors that affect motivation, which include not only the particular features of the immediate situation and task but also the culture(s) an individual resides in, personal characteristics (e.g., gender, race / ethnicity, socioeconomic status, etc.), and past personal experiences related to the achievement activity. Viewing features of the situation as integral to the motivational processes and outcomes mirrors similar approaches in the cognitive and learning sciences that have also focused on the importance of the situation for cognitive activity and for learning and transfer outcomes (Greeno & MMAP, 1997, 1998; Lave, 1988; Lave & Wegner, 1991). We believe that the situated perspective is especially relevant to our present goal of understanding motivation in a particular situation and context—in this case, physicians’ engagement with continuing certification assessments. There are several different features of this context that are important to motivation, including both the particular prior educational and assessment experiences of physicians in the USA (e.g., attending medical school, certification tests) as well as the stereotypes associated with the profession (e.g., about who can be a physician or what resources are required to succeed).

We expand certain components of the situated theory of expectancy-value to accommodate relevant cognitive research. For example, as we elaborate upon below, previous work has focused on expectancies of success, but here we broaden the scope of the term and include other expectancies, such as expectancies of being tested, which also plays an important role in the cognitive literature on memory and learning. We highlight where there are variations—conceptual broadening or narrowing—from the situated expectancy-value concepts and features.

Further, we review and include additional motivational theories and ideas that we believe are of particular relevance to the context of physicians maintaining their expertise and completing continuing assessment. These include achievement goals (given that medical training and performance contexts often have a strong focus on mastery and performance), mindsets and stereotype threat (given varying beliefs about the profession and who can succeed), and test anxiety (given that we are focusing on continued assessment that often takes the form of high-stakes standardized tests).

Another reason to bring these different motivational ideas together into a single framework is to help further connect relevant motivational theories and ideas whose relations are often not explicitly discussed in the literature but whose features and processes often overlap and relate to one another. There have been many calls over the years to integrate these different frameworks or to compare and contrast them (e.g., Hattie et al., 2020). Although our goal here is to review those that we view as particularly relevant to physicians and the maintenance of certification, by expanding the situated expectancy-value framework to relevant cognitive research on learning and testing, we connect these strands of prior research backFootnote 1 together in the hope that future theorizing will further integrate cognitive and motivational theories of learning and performance.

In Fig. 1, we present a model in which we bring together each of the motivational factors of interest, their hypothesized interrelations, and the motivation to learn. We begin by reviewing research on the effects of one’s expectancy for passing the test and of self-efficacy beliefs on engagement and learning outcomes. We then review how learners perceive the benefits of testing and explore the hypothesis that physicians will experience stronger motivation and learning to the degree that the assessment aligns with and confers value to them. We then consider related research on mindsets, which in the context of situated expectancy-value theory, can be viewed as ability beliefs that can influence expectancies and values. We also incorporate achievement goals as both a factor affected by expectancies and values as well as a mediator of the motivation to learn. Next, we discuss the potential perceived costs of testing, such as text anxiety and stereotype threat (i.e., a situation in which one is concerned about potentially confirming a negative stereotype related to an aspect of their identity), as well as approaches to mitigate those costs. We end with a discussion of directions for future work in the areas of motivation and the development of medical expertise.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Situated expectancy-value model of the motivation to learn and interrelations to mindsets, stereotype threat, and achievement goals. The model is adapted from Eccles, Wigfield, and colleagues (Eccles & Wigfield, 2020; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). Positive relations are denoted by the solid arrows and negative relations are denoted by dashed arrows

Expectancies

Expectations of success affect how one studies and performs

A learner’s beliefs about the likelihood of success on a given task has important consequences for their learning activities. Such expectancy beliefs are theorized to be informed by both beliefs related to task outcome success (i.e., outcome expectancies) as well as beliefs about one’s personal capabilities to perform the task (i.e., self-efficacy; Bandura, 1997). In the situated expectancy-value model, a learner’s beliefs about the likelihood of success on a given task affects their motivation to learn. For example, if a learner is given a task that they perceive themselves as unlikely to succeed on (e.g., the test is extremely difficult or insufficient time is given), then they will be less likely to engage in that activity or to prepare adequately because they expect that they will likely fail anyway. Although high failure rates are not traditionally a problem for continuing education programs, what constitutes subjective perceptions of success and failure may be defined differently for different physicians. Prior work has shown that expectancy beliefs have a large impact on academic performance (Meece et al., 1990, EL: 5; Penk & Schipolowski, 2015, EL: 5; Priess-Groben & Hyde, 2017, EL: 5; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000, EL: 2), persistence (Scheier & Carver, 1982, EL: 4), and choice (Bong, 2001, EL: 5; Durik et al., 2006, EL: 5; Simpkins et al., 2006, EL: 5).

Conversely, high self-efficacy is associated with more productive learning behaviors (Bouffard-Bouchard et al., 1991, EL: 5; Parajes, 2008, EL: 2; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990, EL: 5; Schunk & Parajes, 2002, EL: 2). For example, students who have high self-efficacy beliefs are more likely to engage in self-regulated learning and to persist in trying to learn even in the face of difficulties or challenges (Bandura, 1997, EL: 2; Schunk & Parajes, 2002, EL: 2). These beliefs predict student retention and academic performance in school settings (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016, EL: 1) even after controlling for prior knowledge (Bailey et al., 2017, EL: 5; Kalender et al., 2020, EL: 5). A number of factors have been hypothesized to influence the development of self-efficacy, including performance feedback (e.g., test scores), observations of others, social persuasion messages, and physiological states (Bandura, 1997, EL: 2; Britner, 2008, EL: 2; Britner & Parajes, 2006, EL: 2; Usher & Pajares, 2008, EL: 2).

This research on self-efficacy has several implications for continuing assessment of medical expertise. First, an assessment must strike a balance of difficulty in that it is perceived as challenging enough to motivate constructive study activities to prepare for that test, but not so difficult that there would be no possibility for success. One way to communicate the level of difficulty is to provide representative examples of the test items to practice and receive feedback on. Second, because self-efficacy beliefs have a strong impact on how learners prepare and engage with the study materials, continuing certification programs have an opportunity to contribute to the positive development of self-efficacy. That is, the results of the assessment provide a form of performance feedback that could directly impact a physician’s self-efficacy belief (e.g., getting a higher score and thereby increasing self-efficacy). If continuing certification programs transition to more regularly spaced assessments, that provides a further opportunity to develop self-efficacy by providing multiple pieces of feedback over time. Each piece of feedback is an opportunity for an individual to adjust their appraisal of self-efficacy to be more in line with their performance (i.e., to move up or down depending on performance). Providing repeated performance feedback creates an opportunity to determine whether such feedback leads to more accurate self-assessment.

Expectations of test difficulty affect engagement and performance

While the expectancies component of the situated expectancy-value model focuses on expectancies for success on a task, as we discuss above, we also consider here how the expectation that one will be tested in the future can itself create the opportunity to engage in productive study and learning activities. Although just knowing about the existence of an upcoming assessment does not necessarily promote learning (Hyde & Jenkins, 1973, EL: 4; Postman, 1964, EL: 3), an expectation of being tested in a particular way or on certain types of material can lead to better learning (McDaniel et al., 1994, EL: 3; Szpunar et al., 2007, EL: 4). The anticipated difficulty of the test matters, too. For example, laboratory researchers have often examined test difficulty by contrasting a recall task, in which learners must bring to mind the required information (e.g., a fill-in-the-blank item or essay), with a recognition task, in which learners must merely identify the information when it is presented (e.g., multiple-choice or true–false items). All other things being equal, recall is more difficult than recognition. Thus, people expecting a difficult recall test learn and remember more than people expecting an easier recognition test, regardless of the type of test they actually receive (Balota & Neely, 1980, EL: 4; Connor, 1977, EL: 3; d’Ydewalle et al., 1983, EL: 4; Hall et al., 1976, EL: 3; Leonard & Whitten, 1983, EL: 3; Maisto et al., 1977, EL: 4; Neely & Balota, 1981, EL: 4; Schmidt, 1988, EL: 4; c.f., Finley & Benjamin, 2012, EL: 3).

What accounts for this test expectancy effect? Benefits of intentional encoding (i.e., with the goal to learn) appear to be driven largely by the activities that learners engage in when preparing for a test (Hyde & Jenkins, 1969, EL: 4; Hyde & Jenkins, 1973, EL: 4; c.f., Neely & Balota, 1981, EL: 4). Learners expecting a more difficult test can engage in more effective study behaviors, such as studying longer (d’Ydewalle et al., 1983, EL: 4; Thiede, 1996, EL: 3), continuing to practice after an initial quiz (Szpunar et al., 2007, EL: 4), and/or engaging in deeper, more meaningful practice (Hall et al., 1976, EL: 3; Leonard & Whitten, 1983, EL: 3; Schmidt, 1988, EL: 4). Conversely, even offering financial incentives does not increase learning when learners are required to use ineffective learning strategies (Craik & Tulving, 1975, EL: 4).

The results reviewed above suggest that physicians learn and retain more when they expect to be tested on the knowledge and skills they are developing. This is especially the case when the perceived difficulty of the test is difficult enoughFootnote 2 to engender deeper, more effective preparation and when the environment guides physicians to effective study behaviors and activities to capitalize on their increased motivation.

Perceived benefits: what is the value of the test to the learner?

In the situated expectancy-value model, the perceived value of a given task or assessment plays a critical role in motivation to prepare for and engage with it. Value is hypothesized to consist of three distinct components, each of which we review in turn (Wigfield et al., 2016).

Intrinsic task value

Intrinsic task value is interest in a task or assessment for its own sake. Theories of interest typically discuss two different kinds: situational and individual (Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000; Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Krapp, 1999; Schiefele, 1991). Situational interest is hypothesized to be a momentary experience that is driven by environmental factors (e.g., a loud noise) and correlated with both cognitive (e.g., attention) and affective (e.g., surprise) factors (Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000, EL: 2). Individual interest is hypothesized to be a longer-lasting engagement and is associated with one’s knowledge, values, and feelings about the particular topic or task (Renninger, 2000, EL: 2). One perspective on how intrinsic task value emerges is given by discrepancy theory, which posits that learners are motivated to increase a valued competency when they perceive a discrepancy between their current skill and a given goal or standard (Fox & Miner, 1999; see also regulatory focus theory, Higgins, 1997, 2012). Laboratory studies have confirmed that people are sensitive to gaps between perceived and desired knowledge (Dunlosky & Hertzog, 1998, EL: 5; Son & Metcalfe, 2000, EL: 3; Tullis & Benjamin, 2010, EL: 2).

Much prior work has shown that individual interest in the task can increase self-reported effort (Renninger & Hidi, 2002, EL: 5), positive self-regulation (O’Keefe & Linenbrink-Garcia, 2014, EL: 5; Renninger & Hidi, 2019, EL: 2), and deep strategy use (Schiefele et al., 1995, EL: 5). It is also associated with better grades in school (Harackiewicz et al., 2008, EL: 5; Schiefele et al., 1992, EL: 1). Intrinsic task value may also be linked to achievement goals for a particular task, as we elaborate upon below. In the domain of medicine, medical students’ interests and perceived competence have been shown to predict important career choices, such as medical specialty decisions (Williams et al., 1997, EL: 5). Other research has shown that experimental interventions to increase intrinsic task value can facilitate interest and subsequent learning. For example, testing with feedback, in addition to directly enhancing learning, can also increase the desire to learn more about a topic (Abel & Bäuml, 2020: EL 3). This finding aligns with discrepancy theory in that learners need to become aware of (i.e., perceive) discrepancies between actual and desired knowledge to become highly motivated.

Interest and task performance can also be increased by personalizing content (Bernacki & Walkington, 2018, EL: 4; Walkington & Bernacki, 2018, EL: 2). This research implies that the more the content of the test (e.g., topics and patient scenarios) matches the interests of the physician, the more motivated they will be to learn and keep current. It would be desirable to collect information on physicians’ medical interests and personal practice to match those interests, or perhaps to allow physicians some opportunities to select relevant topic areas or problem scenarios to be tested on. Another technique to potentially help select material in a longitudinal spaced-repetition paradigm would be to collect ratings of relevance and use them to prioritize content which information is re-presented.

Utility value

Another aspect of value is called utility value, or the degree to which preparing for and taking the test is useful for accomplishing some valued outcome; that is, as a means to an end (Eccles, 2009; Wigfield & Eccles, 2002). Often, these valued outcomes are broader personal, educational, or professional goals. Correlational research has shown that utility value is positively associated with engagement, learning, and performance outcomes, such as higher grades (Harackiewicz et al., 2016, EL: 2; Harackiewicz et al., 2014, EL: 2). Further, intervention studies have shown that, when utility value increases, so does academic performance (Harackiewicz & Priniski, 2018, EL: 2; Harackiewicz et al., 2016a, 2016b, EL: 4, Hulleman et al., 2010, EL: 4).

Utility value is relevant to longitudinal assessment of medical expertise in at least two ways. The first concerns the usefulness of preparing to take the assessment. That is, does a physician view preparing for the assessment as a helpful activity that contributes to their medical training and skill development more generally, or just something they do because they have to? The more a physician sees connections between the activities of studying and their broader professional goals (e.g., acquiring critical new knowledge), the more motivated they will be to study. The second concerns the value ascribed to the test itself. That is, does a physician view the test as useful to achieving broader educational goals (e.g., staying current) and professional goals (e.g., staying employed, being promoted)?

Interviews with physicians preparing for and taking high stakes tests show a range of perceptions of how relevant and related the content is to their current practice (Chesluk et al., 2019a, 2019b, EL: 5). The work we reviewed above implies that such variation in perceptions is likely to affect physicians' motivation to learn. If physicians see the activity of studying between longitudinal assessment sessions as relevant to their broader professional goals, they will be more motivated and more deeply engaged with the material. Alternatively, if they view the assessment as unrelated and disconnected, they may engage only superficially. Fortunately, some evidence suggests that utility value is amenable to intervention; for instance, in academic settings, it can be improved by having the learner briefly write about the usefulness of the class or discipline to them (Harackiewicz & Priniski, 2018, EL: 2). Feedback within an assessment can also promote utility value. Some longitudinal assessment platforms require the participant to rate each question’s relevance to their medical practice. Periodically providing feedback (e.g., as summary feedback between assessment sessions) regarding questions that a learner missed but that they also rated as relevant to their practice may provide additional motivation for them to review those concepts.

Attainment value

The third component of value is attainment value, or the importance of doing well on a given task or assessment. In the current context, attainment value would capture how important it is to the individual to prepare for the assessment and perform well on it. This judgment will depend on the physician’s perception of what the assessment measures (e.g., relevant medical knowledge and skills), how accurately it measures those competences, and the ramifications of passing or failing the assessment (e.g., often required for preferred employment).

Attainment value is theorized to have implications for one’s self-concept and identity (Eccles, 2009; Eccles & Wigfield, 2020; Ryan & Deci, 2020). For example, self-determination theory implies that performance outcomes provide “data” that can be used to confirm or deny aspects of one’s identity (La Guardia, 2009; Ryan & Deci, 2020), including three core needs of autonomy, relatedness, and—most critical to our purposes—competence. If one perceives the assessment as measuring critical medical competence and performs well, that result can be interpreted as confirming one’s view of oneself as a competent, expert physician. Alternatively, if one perceives the assessment as important but performs poorly on it, it could call into question either one’s view of oneself as an expert, knowledgeable physician, or the validity and accuracy of the test.

Attainment value has been shown to be positively related to engagement (Putwain et al., 2019, EL: 5), effort (Guo et al., 2016, EL: 5), self-concept (Arens et al., 2019, EL: 5), and academic achievement (Trautwein et al., 2012, EL: 5; Meyer et al., 2019; EL: 5). Laboratory research on memory and learning also supports the relevance of attainment value. In one lab paradigm, each to-be-learned item is experimentally assigned a point value that learners are awarded for successful retention, and learners are tasked with earning as many points as possible. Learners consistently remember more of the high-value items, demonstrating that value guides priorities for learning and retention (Castel et al., 2002, EL: 3; Castel et al., 2011, EL: 4; Castel et al., 2013, EL: 3; Hennessee et al., 2018, EL: 3; McGillivray & Castel, 2017, EL: 3).

This work implies that physicians’ perception of the importance of the task and test affects their motivation to learn. The more that physicians see the test as measuring an important set of skills and knowledge, the more time and effort they will invest in performing well on the test. Further, if the assessment provides feedback relevant to physicians’ self-concepts and identities (e.g., their identity as a skilled medical doctor), they will show higher investment in developing their skills and performing well on the assessment. Lastly, longitudinal assessments of medical expertise could encourage physicians to learn and retain particular skills by assigning them higher value or by apportioning more questions to these topics within the assessment (as is often already done).

Growth mindsets promote motivation and learning

Another important motivational factor that can impact how learners prepare for and engage with assessments is their mindset and beliefs about ability. Mindset is a broad term used to describe a set of beliefs that can impact one’s expectations, meaning-making, and behaviors (Dweck & Yeager, 2019, EL: 2).

One of the most powerful mindsets that has been investigated is people’s beliefs about intelligence. Within the situated expectancy-value model of Eccles and Wigfield (2020), mindsets regarding intelligence are captured as part of the self-concept of one’s abilities that can influence expectancies and values. Carol Dweck and her colleagues have been some of the leading researchers on mindsets about intelligence and have focused on two types of beliefs. The first is a belief that intelligence is malleable and can change with experience in a domain, which has been called a growth mindset. The other is a belief that intelligence is inherited and cannot be changed through experience, which has been called a fixed mindset. Intelligence mindsets and ability beliefs have been hypothesized to affect a learner’s expectancies, values, and achievement goals, which in turn affect the motivation to learn (Fig. 1). For example, a growth mindset is hypothesized to lead to positive self-regulated learning behaviors, such as effort in the context of challenge, which in turn lead to better learning and achievement outcomes (Blackwell et al., 2007, EL: 4&5).

A growth mindset predicts positive academic achievement (Costa & Faria, 2018, EL: 1; Blackwell et al., 2007, EL: 4; Gunderson et al., 2013, EL: 5, Henderson & Dweck, 1990, EL: 2, Paunesku et al., 2015, EL: 4; cf. Li & Bates, 2019, EL: 4). Growth and fixed mindsets also relate to students’ self-reported interest (Haimovitz et al., 2011, EL: 5), effort (Blackwell et al., 2007, EL: 5; Miele et al., 2011, EL: 5; Miele & Molden, 2010, EL: 3), and learning goals (Blackwell et al., 2007, EL: 5; Haimovitz et al., 2011, EL: 5). For example, at a correlational level, a growth mindset during the middle-school years predicts learning goals (e.g., “An important reason why I do my school work is because I like to learn new things”) and positive effort beliefs (e.g., “The harder you work at something, the better you will be at it”), which in turn predict positive study strategies (e.g., “I would spend more time studying for tests”) and performance (e.g., achievement test scores) (Blackwell et al., 2007, EL: 5). There is some evidence that the link between growth mindset and academic achievement is causal: Interventions designed to promote growth mindsets, with messages that portray intelligence as malleable with experience and training, lead to positive changes in motivational and achievement outcomes (Blackwell et al., 2007, Expt. 2, EL: 4; Mueller & Dweck, 1998, EL: 4; Yeager, et al., 2016, EL: 4; c.f. Li & Bates, 2019).

In sum, mindsets about intelligence can have powerful downstream effects on motivational and learning outcomes and can directly impact expectancies, values, and goals. Thus, physicians who believe their intelligence and skills are malleable may be more likely to adopt good learning behaviors and goals, which would further their retention of cognitive skills. Physicians’ adoption of a growth mindset may be fostered by the shift in continuing certification toward more regular spaced testing, which provides the opportunity to improve over time.

Achievement goals and the benefits of pursuing mastery

Achievement goals are the reasons why people engage in study and test activities. Achievement goals are sometimes described and investigated separately from expectancy-value theory, but sometimes are included in an overarching model (Plante et al., 2013). In the situated expectancy-value model of Eccles and Wigfield (2020), long- and short-term goals are described as factors that can influence expectancies for success and subjective task values. Others have hypothesized the converse: that expectancies and values affect the adoption of achievement goals (Elliot, 1999; Greene et al., 2004). Some empirical work supports this second view by showing that expectancies and values have both direct effects on motivation for learning and indirect effects through achievement goals (Plante et al., 2013, EL: 5). We incorporate this second view into our model depicted in Fig. 1.

Achievement goals can either be mastery-oriented, with a focus on improving and understanding the material in comparison to one’s prior understanding, or performance-oriented, with a focus on demonstrating ability in comparison to others (Dweck, 1986; Elliot, 1999). Each of these two goals can be approach-or avoidance-based (Elliot, 1999). Approach-based goals are defined by striving toward a positive outcome, and avoidance-based goals are defined by avoiding negative outcomes. Combining these different dimensions results in four different goals: a mastery-approach goal to learn as much as possible, a mastery-avoidance goal to avoid loss of knowledge or skills, a performance-approach goal to perform better than others, and a performance-avoidance goal not to perform worse than others (Elliot & McGregor, 2001; Elliot & Murayama, 2008). We thus view achievement goals as particularly relevant for the context of developing and maintaining medical expertise given the focus on mastery and performance in training and assessment.

Although there has been little work specifically examining achievement goals in the context of practicing physicians, many laboratory experiments and classroom studies have examined these four achievement goals in relation to engagement, learning, and performance outcomes. This literature has consistently linked performance-avoidance goals to negative outcomes, such as poor performance (e.g., grades and tests), as well as low self-efficacy, poor study habits, and procrastination (Elliot & Church, 1997, EL: 5; Elliot & McGregor, 1999, EL: 5; Elliot et al., 1999, EL: 5). In contrast, mastery-approach goals have been consistently associated with positive outcomes, such as self-reported interest and engagement (Elliott & Dweck, 1988, EL: 4; Elliot et al., 1999, EL: 5; Harackiewicz et al., 2002a, 2002b, EL: 5; Harackiewicz et al., 2008, EL: 5) and learning and transfer (Belenky & Nokes-Malach, 2012, 2013, EL: 5). The fact that mastery-approach goals have been related to knowledge transfer is promising for learning in medical education contexts in that it may help physicians acquire sought-after skills critical to adaptive medical expertise (Mylopoulos et al., 2018, EL: 2).

Performance-approach goals correspond to a more intermediate level of performance; they have been related to some positive outcomes, such as better grades and exam performance (Harackiewicz et al., 2002a, 2002b, EL: 2; Linnenbrink-Garcia et al., 2008, EL: 2), but also some negative outcomes, such as less effective self-reported study behaviors (i.e., rote memorization) (Midgley et al., 1996, EL: 5; Senko et al., 2011, EL: 2).

Lastly, mastery-avoidance goals have been the least studied of the four goal types but may be particularly relevant to certification boards as they pertain to avoiding the loss of knowledge and skills that were previously mastered. These goals have been associated with mixed results (Hulleman et al., 2010, EL: 4; Linbrink et al., 2008, EL: 2), including both positive outcomes, such as learning (Richey & Nokes-Malach, 2013, EL: 3), and negative outcomes, such as self-reported test anxiety (Elliot & McGregor, 2001, EL: 5).

How do learners come to adopt one type of achievement goal or another? A number of factors can influence achievement goals (Ames, 1992, EL: 2). Prior experimental and classroom work has shown that instructions can affect the goals that learners adopt in the moment (Elliot & Harackiewiz, 1996, EL: 3; Elliot & Dweck, 1988, EL: 4; Graham & Golan, 1991, EL: 3). For example, telling students that the purpose for a given task is either to “develop their ability or skill and learn from mistakes” or conversely “to compare ability to others and to determine whether they are better or worse than others” can impact learning outcomes and task engagement (Bereby-Meyer & Kaplan, 2005, EL: 3; Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996, EL: 3). Other work has shown that the type of task can also impact the types of goals adopted. For example, a discovery task, in which the learner aims to find a principle that explains a data pattern, has been shown to promote the adoption of mastery-approach goals relative to a task presented as direct instruction followed by practice (Belenky & Nokes-Malach, 2012, EL: 4). The framing of the task is particularly relevant for the continuing certification of medical expertise because the instructions could easily be written to facilitate the adoption of a mastery goal. For example, physicians could be asked to focus on developing their understanding and trying to improve their score over time—aiming to achieve their personal best.

Perceived costs of testing

General aspects of psychological cost

In the situated expectancy-value framework, an important subcomponent of the motivation to learn is the perceived cost of the study activity or test. This component of the model has historically received less attention than expectancy and other aspects of valueFootnote 3 (i.e., intrinsic, utility, attainment); however, more recently, several efforts have been made to develop measurement tools that capture important aspects of cost (Conley, 2012; Flake et al., 2015; Trautwein et al., 2012) and to better understand its role in the expectancy-value framework (Barron & Hulleman, 2015; Eccles & Wigfield, 2020). Four aspects of cost have been identified (Flake et al., 2015). Task effort refers to the amount of time and energy of performing a task itself. Outside effort refers to the amount of time and energy required for other tasks than the focal task (e.g., family and work obligations), which may result in the perception of not having enough time to dedicate to the focal task. The loss of valued alternatives refers to what one has to give up to prepare for the task or test. For example, in the current context, a valued alternative lost in preparing for continuing certification program assessments may be leisure time or family time (Galla et al., 2015: EL 5; Kurzban et al., 2013). The last aspect is emotional cost, which refers to the potential stress and worry caused by the task. For example, anxiety in anticipation of a high-stakes test would increase the perceived emotional cost of the test, as we discuss in further detail below. In interviews with physicians about how they prepared for and took continuing certification examinations, the lack of time available because of outside effort involved in studying and the loss of valued alternatives emerged as important themes (Chesluk et al., 2019a, 2019b, EL: 5).

In the situated expectancy-value model, the more of these perceived costs, the less likely one is to be motivated to learn and prepare for the test. Prior work has shown that perceptions of cost predict additional variation in motivation and performance outcomes above and beyond expectancies and values (Jiang et al., 2018; EL 5; Perez et al., 2014; EL 5). In principle, then, the more that perceived costs can be reduced, the stronger an individual’s motivation to learn. A recent intervention that aimed to reduce cost in an introductory physics course by focusing on the normalization and temporary nature of effort costs has shown some promising results in reducing subsequent perceived costs and increased class performance (Rosenzweig et al., 2020, EL: 4; c.f. Rosenzweig et al., 2022, EL: 4). This work suggests that one way to mitigate the perceived costs of testing would be to discuss those costs in advance in an effort to normalize them.

Another aspect of continuing certification programs that may impact one’s perceptions of cost are the monetary costs associated with certification. There is no research that we know of that has investigated the impact of financial cost of tests on perceived costs within the situated expectancy-value framework, but this could be a useful direction for future work.

Test anxiety

As we discuss above, one form of cost in the situated expectancy-value is emotional cost, which includes anxiety. Indeed, there is a substantial literature specifically on test anxiety, which we review here because it is relevant to the context of continued assessment. Test anxiety is a multi-faceted construct consisting of physiological, psychological (e.g., emotional, cognitive), and behavioral components (Zeidner, 1998, 2009; von der Embse, 2018). It is hypothesized to emerge as worries or fear about a negative evaluation in relation to an evaluative test. Several mechanistic models of test anxiety have been proposed and tested over time, including interference (Alpert & Haber, 1960, EL: 5; Liebert & Morris, 1967, EL: 5), deficit (Tobias, 1985, EL: 2), and transactional models that incorporate components of the former two (Spielberger & Vagg, 1995, EL: 2; see von der Embse, 2018, EL: 1 for a review). More recently, biopsychosocial models have been proposed that focus on the interactive relations between biological, psychological, and social/environmental factors that trigger test anxiety in-the-moment (Segool et al., 2014, EL: 5; Jamieson, 2017, EL: 2).

Although some arousal may be good, many individuals approach standardized tests with levels of anxiety that are high enough to impair performance (von der Embse, 2018, EL: 1). Famously, the Yerkes-Dodson law of arousal and performance states that a moderate level of arousal leads to optimal performance (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908, EL: 3). This “inverted U” model predicts poor performance at low levels of arousal because one is not adequately alert or engaged with the task and at high levels of arousal because one may experience anxiety and worry that interfere with performance. High levels of arousal, anxiety, and worry have been investigated broadly across physical skills and performances as well as intellectual and academic contexts (Alpert & Haber, 1960, EL: 5; Beilock & Carr, 2001, EL: 3; Beilock et al., 2017, EL: 2; Mandler & Sarason, 1952, EL: 4; Sarason, 1980, EL: 2). Test anxiety, in particular, is associated with poorer performance on classroom tests, GPA, IQ tests, and standardized tests (Ackerman & Heggedstad, 1997, EL: 1; Hembree, 1988, EL: 1; von der Embse, 2018, EL: 1).

One reason that high levels of anxiety may be harmful to test-taking is that anxiety can reduce working memory resources (Beilock, 2008, EL: 2; Beilock & Carr, 2005, EL: 4; Moran, 2016, EL: 1). Moran (2016, EL: 1) examined the relationship between self-reported anxiety and working memory capacity in a meta-analysis (N = 22,061 participants) and found a small to moderate negative relationship (Hedges’ g = − 0.33). However, there is still much debate about the boundary conditions of the relations and the exact mechanisms at play. One hypothesis is that anxiety impairs performance via multiple routes: worries impair verbal processing, and high arousal impairs spatial storage (Moran, 2016, EL: 1).

The deleterious effects of anxiety may intensify in response to high-pressure tests. Hinze and Rapp (2014, EL: 3) found that the benefits of testing for learning were diminished if there was significant performance pressure during episodes of memory retrieval. These findings illustrate the importance of reducing pressure during testing in order to maximize learning outcomes. One method that Hinze and Rapp found could reduce pressure was to weigh earlier retrieval-practice questions less than later questions so that learners who do poorly early on can identify areas of weakness and improve upon them. Other work has found that practicing for an assessment using retrieval practice can reduce test anxiety (Agarwal et al., 2014, EL: 3). Thus, the development of a longitudinal assessment focused on learning benefits may reduce the perceived pressure of a one-time, high-stakes test.

Stereotype threat

In the situated expectancy-value framework, stereotypes are described as operating in a broader cultural milieu that is theorized to impact subsequent self-perceptions and task expectancies and values. Here, we elaborate on these effects and connect them to the broader literature on stereotype threat, another phenomenon that can be triggered in high-stakes testing.

Stereotype threat refers to the diminished performance that can occur when reminded of a negative stereotype in a domain in which one otherwise identifies and has high competence (Steele, 1997). It has been observed in varying populations and domains, including women in math, Black Americans in higher education, White males in sports, and older adults in their episodic memory (Barber & Mather, 2013: EL: 3, Bouazzaoui et al., 2020, EL: 5; Nguyen & Ryan, 2008, EL: 1; Rahhal et al., 2001: EL: 3; Steele & Aronson, 1995, EL: 5; Stone et al., 1999, EL: 5).

Stereotype threat is thought to occur because actors of a stereotyped group may exhibit poorer performance as a consequence of not wanting to reinforce the stereotype. Multiple mediating mechanisms have been proposed for this underperformance, including the depletion of working memory resources being consumed to suppress negative thoughts, interference from attending to cognitive processes that are typically automatic, and strategies to protect one’s self-concept (e.g., self-handicapping), among others (Spencer et al., 2016, EL: 2). Shewach et al., (2019, EL: 1) found that when motivational incentives, such as a monetary reward, are present, stereotype threat is much less pronounced than when they are absent (Cohen’s ds 0.14 vs. 0.41, respectively), suggesting that motivation plays an important role in stereotype threat.

Stereotype threat is likely to be applicable to physicians in a continuing assessment given that this context meets the criteria of a domain with which the learners identifies and has high competence (Steele, 1997). Physicians are likely to be highly identified with the medical domain and view themselves as having high competence given their extensive education and training; further, the context of continuing assessment may be viewed as providing results that bear on their evaluation of that competence. However, there are also negative stereotypes associated with particular medical subdisciplines regarding social identities of gender (Fassiotto et al., 2018: EL: 5; Myers et al., 2020: EL: 4) and race and ethnicity (Bullock et al., 2020: EL: 5).

Given the evidence for stereotype threat in testing, a few recommendations for longitudinal assessments may be beneficial. First, stereotype threat is more likely to occur when a test is more difficult (Nguyen & Ryan, 2008, EL: 1; Shewach et al., 2019, EL: 1). Thus, framing any longitudinal assessment in terms of its learning benefits may serve to lower anxiety and reduce perceptions of a test as “high-stakes” (see “reconstrual interventions” in Spencer et al., 2016; EL: 2). Second, in situations where demographic information must be collected, this should occur after any testing or at some other time not directly before the assessment to reduce potentially activating negative stereotypes related to the individuals’ demographics. Third, testing materials should include a diverse cast of characters and should be carefully reviewed so as not to reinforce stereotypes through the testing content.

Relations between features of the model

The situated expectancy-value framework theorizes that relations between expectancies and values have multiplicative effects on motivation and performance. That is, these components are not just additive but interact with one another. Some prior empirical work has found evidence to support this view in that interactive effects of measures of expectancy and value predicted additional variance in motivation and performance above and beyond expectancy and value alone (Nagengast et al., 2011: EL: 4; Trautwein et al., 2012: EL 4). However, much remains to be discovered as to how expectancy and value interact with one another and with cost.

Proposed studies and future directions

Measuring motivation

There is a lack of research on the effects of motivation in longitudinal testing scenarios, including continuing certification programs. One reason for this may be that, to measure motivation in this context, one must determine not only what components of motivation to measure but how to measure them.

There are many potential methods to measure aspects of motivation, including self-report surveys, interviews, behaviors (e.g., choice, time spent, errors, etc.), and physiology, among others. We suggest beginning with self-report surveys because researchers have developed validated measures for many of the constructs that we have discussed that can be used in a variety of educational contexts (e.g., self-efficacy, Bandura, 2006; Fencl & Scheel, 2005; interest and value, Linnenbrink-Garcia et al., 2010; Pintrich et al., 1993; cost, Flake et al., 2015; achievement goals, Elliot & Murayama, 2008; mindsets, Dweck, 1999, 2006; test anxiety, Putwain et al., 2021). Self-report surveys are also relatively easy to implement in a longitudinal assessment context. A first step, then, would be to adapt the items to the continuing certification context and conduct validation studies with this new domain and population.

Given such validated instruments, there are many research questions that could then be investigated concerning the role of motivation in longitudinal assessments. We suggest that one place to start would be to assess whether the motivational components reviewed in this paper—expectancies, perceived values and costs, achievement goals, and mindsets—predict performance in the assessments. Measuring these components of motivation would contribute to basic science by characterizing motivation in the medical field and, more broadly, in an area where individuals have much more expertise in a domain than more novice populations. Such data would be relevant to theory testing and generalization and to understanding relations among motivational constructs. They could also be extremely informative in assessment design decisions and potential interventions. For example, if self-reported utility value strongly predicts performance in the continuing certification context, then interventions (e.g., a brief writing task or instructional framing) could be tested to increase perceptions of utility value, as we elaborate upon below.

In this context, motivation would be relevant both as a process as an outcome. Whether a particular motivational factor, such as utility value, predicts performance in the assessment may shine light onto the specific motivational processes at play when one is taking the assessment. In addition, if it is determined that certain motivational components are particularly important in this context, then measures of motivation could also serve as outcomes or dependent measures for other interventions and assessment changes.

The role of financial cost in a situated expectancy-value framework

As we noted above, there is little or no work examining how the financial cost of taking an assessment may affect motivation, though from the learner’s perspective, these may be an important consideration. Future work should examine how financial costs relate to other cost perceptions and how they affect motivation and performance outcomes.

Interventions to increase motivation and performance

Throughout our review, we identified multiple components of our motivational framework for which past work has developed successful interventions. Such interventions can enhance aspects of value, facilitate productive goals and mindsets, and mitigate potential costs. Here, we describe five that may be particularly well suited to the current context of longitudinal assessments.

Interventions that introduce choice and personalization can increase intrinsic task value and performance (Walkington & Bernacki, 2018, EL: 2; Patall et al., 2008, EL: 1). Thus, allowing individuals some choice of areas to be tested on could increase interest and engagement. Similarly, we hypothesize that personalizing the test to the individual taking it—matching test items to the context or contents of interest—would increase engagement, preparation, and performance outcomes. This would not require any choice within the assessment system itself; the assessment system could automatically assign personalized test content based on an initial survey that the physician would take about their clinical practice.

Second, it might also be possible for interventions to increase the assessment’s perceived utility value. For example, we would predict that motivation to learn could be increased by a 10-min exercise in which individuals write about how the preparation and assessment is relevant to their educational and professional goals.

Third, building on interventions in the broader achievement goal literature (Elliot & Harackiewiz, 1996, EL: 3; Elliot & Dweck, 1988, EL: 4; Graham & Golan, 1991, EL: 3), an assessment’s instructions and structure could help individuals adopt a mastery achievement goal. For example, instructions could emphasize understanding and improvement. The shift toward longitudinal assessment also allows for a focus on intrapersonal comparison; that is, focus on how a physician can improve relative to their past performance rather than other physicians.

Fourth, revising instructions may also provide an avenue to reduce some aspects of perceived costs. Instructions that normalize aspects of the time and effort required to successfully prepare and engage in the longitudinal assessments may reduce perceived cost.

Lastly, discrepancy theory (Fox & Miner, 1999) offers a method for measuring and instilling motivation that, although originally proposed for other forms of continuing medical education, can readily be adapted for longitudinal assessment programs. First, physicians subjectively rate aspects of their clinical competency (e.g., knowledge of diabetes) and the desirability of those aspects (e.g., how important to you is it to have expert knowledge about diabetes?). Then, they take an assessment of the target competency. Finally, a discrepancy score between perceived and actual competency is computed and presented. Physicians who value a particular target competency, but who were unaware of a gap between their perceived and actual ability, may gain an intrinsic desire to improve—especially in the context of a longitudinal assessment program, where they could focus on those topics when studying for the next assessment.

Summary and conclusion

In this paper, we took a situated expectancy-value approach to thinking about the role of motivation in continuing certification programs. We reviewed basic motivational theory and empirical work from laboratory and classroom settings, and we discussed their implications and applications in the context of continuing certification program assessments. This review suggests several motivational benefits of testing as well as some potential challenges posed by high-stakes standardized tests.

Many of the motivational benefits for testing can be understood from the equation of having the perceived benefits of a test outweigh the perceived costs of preparing for and taking it. We found that a sufficiently challenging test can increase both motivation to learn and later performance as long as the test is not perceived as too difficult; that is, if learners perceive that investing effort is likely to increase success on the test. Two ways to make clear the level of difficulty are to describe the specific task items used and to give representative problems to practice and receive feedback on.

We also reviewed three components of value (intrinsic, utility, and attainment) that should be attended to when designing an assessment. The ideal assessment should be perceived as relevant to the practitioner’s interests (e.g., in terms of the topics and scenarios). It should be useful to furthering the practitioner’s educational and professional goals, such as developing expertise and staying current. And, it should be perceived as important; that is, as an accurate measure of medical knowledge and skills and as an opportunity to confirm a physician’s identity as a skilled medical expert. These values can be highlighted in the instructions and framing of the assessment and potentially in preparatory activities that might further reinforce them (e.g., a writing activity to discuss why this assessment is helpful to one’s goals). Similarly, framing the longitudinal assessment and feedback as an opportunity to learn and develop can further facilitate the adoption of mastery-approach goals and growth mindsets.

Complementing efforts to boost perceived value is an effort to mitigate perceived costs. It would be helpful to convey the task effort as reasonable and worthwhile. High-stakes assessment can also carry an emotional cost in the form of test anxiety, but the move to a longitudinal assessment scheme of more frequent testing may reduce test anxiety relative to less frequent, higher-stakes tests. We also discussed the related phenomenon of stereotype threat, which can be mitigated by emphasizing the assessment as an opportunity to improve (as opposed to a high-stakes, evaluative test), highlighting the components of value previously described, asking demographics at the end of the assessment or at some other time not right before the assessment, and including diverse demographic features in the testing clinical scenarios.

By considering how both motivational and cognitive factors relate to the benefits and costs of longitudinal assessment, future work can build theories that integrate across these frameworks and a practical opportunity to design multi-purpose assessments that are both engaging and useful.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. Work on learning and motivation at the beginning of the twentieth century by Thorndike, Hull, and Tolman, among others, closely connected the processes of motivation and learning together within singular theories (see Klein, 2019, for an overview). We hope that future work continues to bring research on cognition and learning and motivation and learning back together in integrated ways.

  2. Of course, we discuss above, if the test is expected to be too difficult and outcome expectations are consequently low, learners may instead disengage from the task entirely.

  3. In the situated expectancy-value framework, cost is considered a subcomponent of value. Here, we represent cost separately from value in Fig. 1 to highlight its role and to both build on the prior work on costs as well as broaden the definition to consider other types of costs (e.g., financial costs of taking the assessment).

References

  • Abel, M., & Bäuml, K.-H.T. (2020). Would you like to learn more? Retrieval practice plus feedback can increase motivation to keep on studying. Cognition, 201, 104316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121(2), 219–245.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Agarwal, P. K., D'Antonio, L., Roediger, H. L. III, McDermott, K. B., & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Classroom-based programs of retrieval practice reduce middle school and high school students’ test anxiety. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(3), 131–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alpert, R., & Haber, R. N. (1960). Anxiety in academic achievement situations. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61(2), 207–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(3), 261–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arens, A., Schmidt, I., & Preckel, F. (2019). Longitudinal relations among self-concept, intrinsic value, and attainment value across secondary school years in three academic domains. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(4), 663–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, J. M., Lombardi, D., Cordova, J. R., & Sinatra, G. M. (2017). Meeting students halfway: Increasing self-efficacy and promoting knowledge change in astronomy. Physical Review Physics Education Research, 13(2), 020140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., & Neely, J. H. (1980). Test-expectancy and word-frequency effects in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(5), 576–587.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In F. Pajaras & T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 307–337). Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013). Stereotype threat can both enhance and impair older adults’ memory. Psychological Science, 24(12), 2522–2529.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, K. E., & Hulleman, C. S. (2015). Expectancy-value-cost model of motivation. In J. S. Eccles & K. Salmelo-Aro (Eds.), International encyclopedia of social and behavioral sciences: Motivational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 261–271). Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L. (2008). Math performance in stressful situations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(5), 393–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 701–725.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail: Working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Science, 16(2), 101–105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., Schaeffer, M. W., & Rozek, C. S. (2017). Understanding and addressing performance anxiety. In A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck, & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation: Theory and application (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belenky, D. M., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2012). Motivation and transfer: The role of mastery-approach goals in preparation for future learning. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(3), 399–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belenky, D. M., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2013). Knowledge transfer and mastery-approach goals: Effects of structure and framing. Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 21–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. S., & Tullis, J. G. (2010). What makes distributed practice effective? Cognitive Psychology, 61, 228–247.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bereby-Meyer, Y., & Kaplan, A. (2005). Motivational influences of problem-solving strategies. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernacki, M., & Walkington, C. (2018). The role of situational interest in personalized learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(6), 864–881.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246–263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bong, M. (2001). Between- and within-domain relations of academic motivation among middle and high school students: Self-efficacy, task value, and achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(1), 23–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouazzaoui, B., Fay, S., Guerrero-Sastoque, L., Semaine, M., Isingrini, M., & Taconnat, L. (2020). Memory age-based stereotype threat: Role of locus of control and anxiety. Experimental Aging Research, 46(1), 39–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bouffard-Bouchard, T., Parent, S., & Larvilee, S. (1991). Influence of self-efficacy on self-regulation and performance among junior and senior high-school age students. International Journal of Behavior Development, 14(2), 153–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britner, S. L. (2008). Motivation in high school science students: A comparison of gender differences in life physical, and earth science classes. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45(8), 955–970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britner, S. L., & Parajes, F. (2006). Sources of science self-efficacy beliefs of middle school students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43(5), 485–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, J. L., Lockspeiser, T., Pino-Jones, A. D., Richards, R., Teherani, A., & Hauer, K. E. (2020). They don’t see a lot of people my color: A mixed methods study of racial/ethnic stereotype threat among medical students on core clerkships. Academic Medicine, 95(11), S58–S66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castel, A. D., Benjamin, A. S., Craik, F. I., & Watkins, M. J. (2002). The effects of aging on selectivity and control in short-term recall. Memory & Cognition, 30(7), 1078–1085.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castel, A. D., Humphreys, K. L., Lee, S. S., Galván, A., Balota, D. A., & McCabe, D. P. (2011). The development of memory efficiency and value-directed remembering across the life span: A cross-sectional study of memory and selectivity. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1553–1564.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Castel, A. D., Murayama, K., Friedman, M. C., McGillivray, S., & Link, I. (2013). Selecting valuable information to remember: Age-related differences and similarities in self-regulated learning. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 232–242.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chesluk, B., Eden, A., Hansen, E., Johnson, M., Reddy, S., Bernabeo, E., & Gray, B. (2019a). How physicians prepare for maintenance of certification exams: A qualitative study. Academic Medicine, 94(12), 1931–1938.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chesluk, B., Gray, B., Eden, A., Hansen, E., Lynn, L., & Peterson, L. (2019b). That was pretty powerful: A qualitative study of what physicians learn when preparing for their Maintenance of Certification exams. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 34(9), 1790–1796.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Conley, A. M. (2012). Patterns of motivation beliefs: Combining achievement goal and expectancy-value perspectives. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 32–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connor, J. M. (1977). Effects of organization and expectancy on recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 5(3), 315–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A., & Faria, L. (2018). Implicit theories of intelligence and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(829), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 268–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (1998). Aging and deficits in associative memory: What is the role of strategy production? Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 597–607.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Task values and ability beliefs as predictors of high school literacy choices: A developmental analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 382–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41(10), 1040–1048.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., & Yeager, D. S. (2019). Mindsets: A view from two eras. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(3), 481–496.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • d’Ydewalle, G., Swerts, A., & De Corte, E. (1983). Study time and test performance as a function of test expectations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8(1), 55–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. (2009). Who am I and what am I going to do with my life? Personal and collective identities as motivators of action. Educational Psychologist, 44(2), 78–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 109–132.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2020). From expectancy-value theory to situated expectancy-value theory: A developmental, social cognitive, and sociocultural perspective on motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61, 101859.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34(3), 169–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(1), 218–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 461–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (1999). Test anxiety and the hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 628–644.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 x 2 Achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(3), 501–519.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Gable, S. (1999). Achievement goals, study strategies, and exam performance: A mediational analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 549–563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Murayama, K. (2008). On the measurement of achievement goals: Critique, illustration, and application. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 613–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(1), 5–12.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fassiotto, M., Li, J., Maldonado, Y., & Kothary, N. (2018). Female surgeons as counter stereotype: The impact of gender perceptions on trainee evaluations of physician faculty. Journal of Surgical Education, 75(5), 1140–1148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fencl, H. S., & Scheel, K. R. (2005). Research and teaching: Engaging students—An examination of the effects of teaching strategies on self-efficacy and course climate in a nonmajors physics course. Journal of College Science Teaching, 35(1), 20–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finley, J. R., & Benjamin, A. S. (2012). Adaptive and qualitative changes in encoding strategy with experience: Evidence from the test-expectancy paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 632–652.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flake, J. K., Barron, K. E., Hulleman, C., McCoach, B. D., & Welsh, M. E. (2015). Measuring cost: The forgotten component of expectancy-value theory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 41, 232–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R. D., & Miner, C. (1999). Motivation and the facilitation of change, learning, and participation in educational programs for health professionals. Journal of Continuing Education in the Health Professions, 19(3), 132–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraundorf, S. H., Caddick, Z. A., Nokes-Malach, T. J., & Rottman, B. M. (2023). Cognitive perspectives on maintaining physicians’ medical expertise: IV. Best practices and open questions in using testing to enhance learning and retention. Principles & Implications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galla, B. M., Plummer, B. D., White, R. E., Meketon, D., D’Mello, S. K., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). The academic diligence task (ADT): Assessing individual differences in effort on tedious but important schoolwork. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39(4), 314–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., & Golan, S. (1991). Motivational influences on cognition: Task involvement, ego involvement, and depth of information processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(2), 187–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, B. A., Miller, R. B., Crowson, H. M., Duke, B. L., & Akey, K. L. (2004). Predicting high school students’ cognitive engagement and achievement: Contributions of classroom perceptions and motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29(4), 462–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G., The Middle-School Mathematics Through Applications Project Group. (1997). Theories and practices in thinking and learning to think. American Journal of Education, 106, 85–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G., The Middle-School Mathematics Through Applications Project Group. (1998). The situativity of knowing, learning, and research. American Psychologist, 53, 5–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunderson, E. A., Gripshover, S. J., Romero, C., Dweck, C. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2013). Parent praise to 1- to 3-year olds predicts children’s motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Development, 84(5), 1526–1541.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guo, J., Nagengast, B., Marsh, H. W., Kelava, A., Gaspard, H., Brandt, H., Cambria, J., Flunger, B., Dicke, A., Hafner, I., Brisson, B., & Trautwein, U. (2016). Probing the unique contributions of self-concept, task values, and their interactions using multiple value facets and multiple academic outcomes. AERA Open, 2(1), 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haimovitz, K., Wormington, S. V., & Corpus, J. H. (2011). Dangerous mindsets: How beliefs about intelligence predict motivational change. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 747–752.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., Grossman, L. R., & Elwood, K. D. (1976). Differences in encoding for free recall vs. recognition. Memory & Cognition, 4(5), 507–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002a). Revision of achievement goal theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(3), 638–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002b). Predicting success in college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(3), 562–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y., Priniski, S. J., & Hyde, J. S. (2016). Closing achievement gaps with a utility-value intervention: Disentangling race and social class. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(5), 745–765.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Durik, A. M., Barron, K. E., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., & Tauer, J. M. (2008). The role of achievement goals in the development of interest: Reciprocal relations between achievement goals, interest, and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 105–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., & Priniski, S. J. (2018). Improving student outcomes in higher education: The science of targeted intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 409–435.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Smith, J. L., & Priniski, S. J. (2016b). Interest matters: The importance of promoting interest in education. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brian Sciences, 3(2), 220–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Tibbetts, Y., Canning, E. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2014). Harnessing values to promote motivation in education. In S. Karabenick & T. Urden (Eds.), Motivational Interventions, advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 18, pp. 71–105). Emerald Group Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hattie, J., Hodis, F. A., & Kang, S. H. K. (2020). Theories of motivation: Integration and ways forward. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61, 101865.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects, and treatment of test anxiety. Review of Educational Research, 58(1), 47–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, V. L., & Dweck, C. S. (1990). Motivation and achievement. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 308–329). Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hennessee, J. P., Knowlton, B. J., & Castel, A. D. (2018). The effects of value on context-item associative memory in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 33(1), 46–56.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational Research, 70(2), 151–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 111–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280–1300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (2012). Regulatory focus theory. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 483–504). Sage Publications Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinze, S. R., & Rapp, D. N. (2014). Retrieval (sometimes) enhances learning: Performance pressure reduces the benefits of retrieval practice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 597–606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Honicke, T., & Broadbent, J. (2016). The influence of academic self-efficacy on academic performance: A systematic review. Educational Research Review, 17, 63–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulleman, C. S., Godes, O., Hendricks, B. L., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). Enhancing interest and performance with a utility value intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 880–895.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2009). Promoting interest and performance in high school science classes. Science, 326(5958), 1410–1412.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, T. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1969). Differential effects of incidental tasks on the organization of recall of a list of highly associated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82(3), 472–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, T. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1973). Recall for words as a function of semantic, graphic, and syntactic orienting tasks. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(5), 471–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, J. P. (2017). Challenge and threat appraisals. In A. Elliot, C. Dweck, & D. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation: Theory and application (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, Y., Rosenzweig, E. Q., & Gaspard, H. (2018). An expectancy-value-cost approach in predicting adolescent students’ academic motivation and achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 54, 139–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalender, Y., Marshman, E., Schunn, C., Nokes-Malach, T. J., & Singh, C. (2020). Damage caused by women’s lowered self-efficacy on physics learning. Physical Review Physics Education Research, 16(1), 010118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, S. B. (2019). Learning. Sage publications Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A. (1999). Interest, motivation and learning: An educational-psychological perspective. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14(1), 23–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). Cost-benefit models as the next, best option for understanding subjective effort. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 707–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Guardia, J. G. (2009). Developing who I am: A self-determination theory approach to the establishment of health identities. Educational Psychologist, 44(2), 90–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, J. M., & Whitten, W. B. (1983). Information stored when expecting recall or recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(3), 440–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leyens, J. P., Desert, M., Croizet, J. C., & Darcis, C. (2000). Stereotype threat: Are lower status and history of stigmatization preconditions of stereotype threat? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(10), 1189–1199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, Y., & Bates, T. C. (2019). You can’t change your basic ability, but you work at things, and that’s how we get hard things done: Testing the role of growth mindset on response to setbacks, educational attainment, and cognitive ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(9), 1640–1655.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 20(3), 975–978.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Durik, A. M., Conley, A. M. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Karabenick, S. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). Measuring situational interest in academic domains. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 70, 647–671.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Tyson, D. F., & Patall, E. A. (2008). When are achievement goal orientations beneficial for academic achievement? A closer look at main effects and moderating factors. Revue Internationale De Psychologie Sociale, 21, 19–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maisto, S. A., Dewaard, R. J., & Miller, M. E. (1977). Encoding processes for recall and recognition: The effect of instructions and auxiliary task performance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9(2), 127–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Sarason, S. B. (1952). A study of anxiety and learning. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47(2), 166–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Blischak, D. M., & Challis, B. (1994). The effects of test expectancy on processing and memory of prose. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19(2), 230–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGillivray, S., & Castel, A. D. (2017). Older and younger adults’ strategic control of metacognitive monitoring: The role of consequences, task, and prior knowledge. Experimental Aging Research, 43(3), 233–256.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990). Predictors of math anxiety and its influence on young adolescents’ course enrollment intentions and performance in mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 60–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J., Fleckenstein, J., & Koller, O. (2019). Expectancy value interactions and academic achievement: Differential relationships with achievement measures. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 58, 58–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Midgley, C., Arunkumar, R., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). “If I don’t do well tomorrow, there’s a reason”: Predictors of adolescents’ use of academic self-handicapping strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 423–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miele, D. B., Finn, B., & Molden, D. C. (2011). Does easily learned mean easily remembered? It depends on your beliefs of intelligence. Psychological Science, 22(3), 320–324.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miele, D. B., & Molden, D. C. (2010). Naive theories of intelligence and the role of processing fluency in perceived comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 535–557.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moran, T. P. (2016). Anxiety and working memory capacity: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 142(8), 831–864.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 33–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, S. P., Dasari, M., Brown, J. B., Lumpkin, S. T., Neal, M. D., Abebe, K. Z., Chaumont, N., Downs-Canner, S. M., Flanagan, M. R., Lee, K. K., & Rosengart, M. R. (2020). Effects of gender bias and stereotypes in surgical training: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Surgery, 155(7), 552–560.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mylopoulos, M., Kulasegaram, K., & Woods, N. N. (2018). Developing the experts we need: Fostering adaptive expertise through education. Journal of Evlauation in Clinical Practice, 24(3), 674–677.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagengast, B., Marsh, H. M., Scalas, L. F., Xu, M. K., Hau, K., & Trautwein, U. (2011). Who took the “x” out of expectancy-value theory? A psychological mystery, a substantive-methodological synergy, and a cross-national generalization. Psychological Science, 22(8), 1058–1066.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H., & Balota, D. A. (1981). Test-expectancy and semantic-organization effects in recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 9(3), 283–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nguyen, H. H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1314–1334.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, P. A., & Linenbrink-Garcia, L. (2014). The role of interest in optimizing performance and self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 70–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pajares, F. (2008). Motivational role of self-efficacy beliefs in self-regulated learning. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 111–139). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: A meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 270–300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., Romero, C., Smith, E. N., Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Mind-set interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underachievement. Psychological Science, 26(6), 784–793.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Penk, C., & Schipolowski, S. (2015). Is it about value? Bringing back the expectancy component to the assessment of test-taking motivation. Learning and Individual Differences, 42, 27–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perez, T., Cromley, J. G., & Kaplan, A. (2014). The role of identity development, values, and costs in college STEM retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1), 315–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53, 801–813.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plante, I., O’Keefe, P. A., & Theoret, M. (2013). The relation between achievement goal and expectancy-value theories in predicting achievement-related outcomes: A test of four theoretical conceptions. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 65–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L. (1964). Studies of learning to learn II. Changes in transfer as a function of practice. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3(5), 437–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Priess-Groben, H. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2017). Implicit theories, expectancies, and values predict mathematics motivation and behavior across high school and college. Journal of Youth Adolescence, 46, 1318–1332.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Putwain, D. W., Nicholson, L. J., Pekrun, R., Becker, S., & Symes, W. (2019). Expectancy of success, attainment value, engagement, and achievement: A moderated mediation analysis. Learning and Instruction, 60, 117–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putwain, D. W., von der Embse, N. P., Rainbird, E. C., & West, G. (2021). The development and validation of a new multidimensional test anxiety scale (MTAS). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 37(3), 236–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rahhal, T. A., Hasher, L., & Colcombe, S. J. (2001). Instructional manipulations and age differences in memory: Now you see them, now you don’t. Psychology and Aging, 16, 697–706.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Renninger, K. A. (2000). Individual interest and its implications for understanding intrinsic motivation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 373–404). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. (2002). Student interest and achievement: Developmental issues raised by a case study. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), A volume in the educational psychology series. Development of achievement motivation (pp. 173–195). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. E. (2019). Interest development and learning. In K. A. Renninger & S. E. Hidi (Eds.), Cambridge handbooks in psychology. The Cambridge handbook of motivation and learning (pp. 265–290). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richey, J. E., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2013). How much is too much? Learning and motivation effects of adding instructional explanations to worked examples. Learning and Instruction, 25, 104–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, E. Q., Song, Y., & Clark, S. (2022). Mixed effects of a randomized trial replication study testing a cost-focused motivational intervention. Learning and Instruction, 82, 101660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, E. Q., Wigfield, A., & Hulleman, C. S. (2020). More useful or not so bad? Examining the effects of utility value and cost reduction interventions in college physics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(1), 166–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2020). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: Definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 101860.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarason, I. G. (Ed.). (1980). Test anxiety: Theory, research, and applications. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1982). Self-consciousness, outcome expectancy, and persistence. Journal of Research in Personality, 16(4), 409–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., Wild, K. P., & Krapp, A. (1995). Course-specific interest and extrinsic motivation as predictors of specific learning strategies and course grades. In 6th EARLI conference in Nijmegen.

  • Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning, and motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26(3–4), 299–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., Krapp, A., & Winteler, A. (1992). Interest as a predictor of academic achievement: A meta-analysis of research. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 183–212). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, S. R. (1988). Test expectancy and individual-item versus relational processing. The American Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 59–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2002). The development of academic self-efficacy. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), A volume in the educational psychology series. Development of achievement motivation (pp. 15–31). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segool, N. K., von der Embse, N. P., Mata, A. D., & Gallant, J. (2014). Cognitive behavioral model of test anxiety in a high stakes context: An exploratory study. School Mental Health, 6, 50–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senko, C., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2011). Achievement goal theory at the crossroads: Old controversies, current challenges, and new directions. Educational Psychologist, 46(1), 26–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shewach, O. R., Sackett, P. R., & Quint, S. (2019). Stereotype threat effects in settings with features likely versus unlikely in operational test settings: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 104(12), 1514–1534.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Math and science motivation: A longitudinal examination of the links between choices and beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Son, L. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2000). Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(1), 204.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, S. J., Logel, C., & Davies, P. G. (2016). Stereotype threat. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 415–437.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., & Vagg, P. R. (1995). Test anxiety: A transactional process model. In C. D. Spielberger & P. R. Vagg (Eds.), Series in clinical and community psychology. Test anxiety: Theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 3–14). Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613–629.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797–811.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, J., Lynch, C. I., Sjomeling, M., & Darley, J. M. (1999). Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1213–1227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar, K. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2007). Expectation of a final cumulative test enhances long-term retention. Memory & Cognition, 35(5), 1007–1013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W. (1996). The relative importance of anticipated test format and anticipated test difficulty on performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 49(4), 901–918.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobias, S. (1985). Test anxiety: Interference, defective skills, and cognitive capacity. Educational Psychologist, 20(3), 135–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trautwein, U., Marsh, H. W., Nagengast, B., Lüdtke, O., Nagy, G., & Jonkmann, K. (2012). Probing for the multiplicative term in modern expectancy–value theory: A latent interaction modeling study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 763.

    Google Scholar 

  • Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy in school: Critical review of the literature and future directions. Review of Educational Research, 78(4), 751–796.

    Google Scholar 

  • von der Embse, N., Jester, D., Roy, D., & Post, J. (2018). Text anxiety effects, predictors, and correlates: A 30 year meta-analytic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 227, 483–493.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walkington, C., & Bernacki, M. L. (2018). Personalization of instruction: Design dimensions and implications for cognition. Journal of Experimental Education, 86(1), 50–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 68–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of competence beliefs and values from childhood through adolescence. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 92–120). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., Tonks, S., & Klauda, S. L. (2016). Expectancy-value theory. In K. R. Wentzel & D. Miele (Eds.), Handbook of motivation in school (2nd ed., pp. 55–74). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C., Saizow, R., Ross, L., & Deci, E. L. (1997). Motivation underlying career choice for internal medicine and surgery. Social Science & Medicine, 45(11), 1705–1713.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). How can we instill productive mindsets at scale? A review of the evidence and an initial R&D agenda. In White paper prepared for white house meeting excellence in education. The Importance of Academic Mindsets.

  • Yeager, D. S., Hanselman, P., & Dweck, C. S. (2019). A national study reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature, 573, 364–369.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., Romero, C., Paunesku, D., Hulleman, C. S., Schneider, B., Hinojosa, C., Lee, H. Y., O'Brien, J., Flint, K., Roberts, A., Trott, J., Greene, D., Walton, G. M., & Dweck, C. S. (2016). Using design thinking to improve psychological interventions: The case of the growth mindset during the transition to high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(3), 374–391.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18(5), 459–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeidner, M. (2009). Test anxiety. In I. B. Weiner & W. E. Craighead (Eds.), The corsini encyclopedia of psychology (pp. 1–3). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Andrew Bazemore, Rebecca S. Lipner, David B. Swanson, and Thomas O’Neill for feedback on earlier drafts of this work.

Funding

This work was funded by a grant from the American Board of Internal Medicine (ABIM), American Board of Medical Specialties (ABMS), and American Board of Family Medicine (ABFM). Individuals from ABIM, ABMS, and ABFM provided feedback on the overall goals of the review and on earlier drafts of the manuscript, but approval of the final manuscript rested with the authors alone.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

TN-M wrote the first draft of the manuscript. SF, ZC, and BR provided feedback. All authors contributed to revising the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Scott H. Fraundorf.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interest

The authors were not involved with the peer review process of this work.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nokes-Malach, T.J., Fraundorf, S.H., Caddick, Z.A. et al. Cognitive perspectives on maintaining physicians’ medical expertise: V. Using a motivational framework to understand the benefits and costs of testing. Cogn. Research 8, 64 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-023-00518-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-023-00518-6

Keywords