Abel, E. L., & Kruger, M. L. (2010). Smile intensity in photographs predicts longevity. Psychological Science, 21(4), 542–544. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363775
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Adams, R. C., Challenger, A., Bratton, L., Boivin, J., Bott, L., Powell, G., Williams, A., Chambers, C. D., & Sumner, P. (2019). Claims of causality in health news: A randomised trial. BMC Medicine, 17(1), 1–11.
Article
Google Scholar
Adams, R. C., Sumner, P., Vivian-Griffiths, S., Barrington, A., Williams, A., Boivin, J., Chambers, C. D., & Bott, L. (2017). How readers understand causal and correlational expressions used in news headlines. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 23(1), 1–14.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ahn, W. K., Kalish, C. W., Medin, D. L., & Gelman, S. A. (1995). The role of covariation versus mechanism information in causal attribution. Cognition, 54(3), 299–352.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ainsworth, S., & Loizou, A. (2003). The effects of self-explaining when learning with text or diagrams. Cognitive Science, 27(4), 669–681. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2706_6
Article
Google Scholar
Ainsworth, S. E., & Scheiter, K. (2021). Learning by drawing visual representations: Potential, purposes, and practical implications. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(1), 61–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420979582
Article
Google Scholar
Amsel, E., Klaczynski, P. A., Johnston, A., Bench, S., Close, J., Sadler, E., & Walker, R. (2008). A dual-process account of the development of scientific reasoning: The nature and development of metacognitive intercession skills. Cognitive Development, 23(4), 452–471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.09.002
Article
Google Scholar
Bao, L., Cai, T., Koenig, K., Fang, K., Han, J., Wang, J., et al. (2009). Learning and scientific reasoning. Science, 323(5914), 586–587.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Baram-Tsabari, A., & Osborne, J. (2015). Bridging science education and science communication research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 52(2), 135–144. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21202
Article
Google Scholar
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., Crowe, D. S., Bernhardt, P., Buckner, C., & Allman, A. L. (2010). Teaching and assessing critical thinking skills for argument analysis in psychology. Teaching of Psychology, 37(2), 91–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/00986281003626656
Article
Google Scholar
Berthold, K., & Renkl, A. (2009). Instructional aids to support a conceptual understanding of multiple representations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(1), 70–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013247
Article
Google Scholar
Berthold, K., & Renkl, A. (2010). How to foster active processing of explanations in instructional communication. Educational Psychology Review, 22(1), 25–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9124-9
Article
Google Scholar
Billman, D., Bornstein, B., & Richards, J. (1992). Effects of expectancy on assessing covariation in data: “Prior belief” versus “meaning.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 53(1), 74–88.
Article
Google Scholar
Blalock, H. M., Jr. (1987). Some general goals in teaching statistics. Teaching Sociology, 15(2), 164–172.
Article
Google Scholar
Bleske-Rechek, A., Gunseor, M. M., & Maly, J. R. (2018). Does the language fit the evidence? Unwarranted causal language in psychological scientists’ scholarly work. The Behavior Therapist, 41(8), 341–352.
Google Scholar
Bleske-Rechek, A., Morrison, K. M., & Heidtke, L. D. (2015). Causal inference from descriptions of experimental and non-experimental research: Public understanding of correlation-versus-causation. Journal of General Psychology, 142(1), 48–70.
Article
Google Scholar
Bobek, E., & Tversky, B. (2016). Creating visual explanations improves learning. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 1, 27. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-016-0031-6
Article
Google Scholar
Bott, L., Bratton, L., Diaconu, B., Adams, R. C., Challenger, A., Boivin, J., Williams, A., & Sumner, P. (2019). Caveats in science-based news stories communicate caution without lowering interest. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 25(4), 517–542.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bromme, R., & Goldman, S. R. (2014). The public’s bounded understanding of science. Educational Psychologist, 49(2), 59–69. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2014.921572
Article
Google Scholar
Brown, A. W., Brown, M. M. B., & Allison, D. B. (2013). Belief beyond the evidence: Using the proposed effect of breakfast on obesity to show 2 practices that distort scientific evidence. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 98, 1298–1308. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.113.064410
Article
Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1957). Going beyond the information given. In J. S. Bruner, E. Brunswik, L. Festinger, F. Heider, K. F. Muenzinger, C. E. Osgood, & D. Rapaport (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to cognition (pp. 41–69). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Reprinted in Bruner, J. S. (1973), Beyond the information given (pp. 218–238). New York: Norton].
Butler, C. S. (1990). Qualifications in science: Modal meanings in scientific texts. In W. Nash (Ed.), The writing scholar: Studies in academic discourse (pp. 137–170). Sage Publications.
Google Scholar
Carnevale, A., Strohl, J., & Smith, N. (2009). Help wanted: Postsecondary education and training required. New Directions for Community Colleges, 146, 21–31. https://doi.org/10.1002/cc.363
Article
Google Scholar
Cheng, P. (1997). From covariation to causation: A causal power theory. Psychological Review, 104, 367–405.
Article
Google Scholar
Chi, M. T. H. (2000). Self-explaining expository texts: The dual process of generating inferences and repairing mental models. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology: Educational design and cognitive science (pp. 161–238). Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13(2), 145–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/0364-0213(89)90002-5
Article
Google Scholar
Chi, M. T., De Leeuw, N., Chiu, M. H., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18(3), 439–477. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1803_3
Article
Google Scholar
Corrigan, R., & Denton, P. (1996). Causal understanding as a developmental primitive. Developmental Review, 16, 162–202.
Article
Google Scholar
Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.01.1.p042
Article
Google Scholar
Crowell, A., & Schunn, C. (2016). Unpacking the relationship between science education and applied scientific literacy. Research in Science Education, 46(1), 129–140. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-015-9462-1
Article
Google Scholar
Dor, D. (2003). On newspaper headlines as relevance optimizers. Journal of Pragmatics, 35(5), 695–721.
Article
Google Scholar
Durant, J. R. (1993). What is scientific literacy? In J. R. Durant & J. Gregory (Eds.), Science and culture in Europe (pp. 129–137). Science Museum.
Google Scholar
Durik, A. M., Britt, M. A., Reynolds, R., & Storey, J. (2008). The effects of hedges in persuasive arguments: A nuanced analysis of language. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 27(3), 217–234.
Article
Google Scholar
Durkin, K., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to support learning about decimal magnitude. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 206–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.001
Article
Google Scholar
Ecker, U. K., Lewandowsky, S., Chang, E. P., & Pillai, R. (2014a). The effects of subtle misinformation in news headlines. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20, 323–335. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000028
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ecker, U. K., Swire, B., & Lewandowsky, S. (2014b). Correcting misinformation—A challenge for education and cognitive science. In D. N. Rapp & J. L. G. Braasch (Eds.), Processing inaccurate information: Theoretical and applied perspectives from cognitive science and the educational sciences (pp. 13–38). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Elwert, F. (2013). Graphical causal models. In S. L. Morgan (Ed.), Handbook of causal analysis for social research, Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-6094-3_13
Evans, D. (2003a). Hierarchy of evidence: A framework for ranking evidence evaluating healthcare interventions. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 12(1), 77–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.001
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Evans, J. (2003b). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(10), 454–469. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012
Article
Google Scholar
Evans, J., & Curtis-Holmes, J. (2005). Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 11(4), 382–389. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780542000005
Article
Google Scholar
Fiorella, L., & Zhang, Q. (2018). Drawing boundary conditions for learning by drawing. Educational Psychology Review, 30(3), 1115–1137. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-018-9444-8
Article
Google Scholar
Fong, G., Krantz, D., & Nisbett, R. (1986). The effects of statistical training on thinking about everyday problems. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 253–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90001-0
Article
Google Scholar
Fugelsang, J. A., & Thompson, V. A. (2000). Strategy selection in causal reasoning: When beliefs and covariation collide. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 13–32.
Article
Google Scholar
Fugelsang, J. A., & Thompson, V. A. (2003). A dual-process model of belief and evidence interactions in causal reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 31, 800–815.
Article
Google Scholar
Gobert, J., & Clement, J. (1999). Effects of student-generated diagrams versus student-generated summaries on conceptual understanding of causal and dynamic knowledge in plate tectonics. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36(1), 39–53.
Article
Google Scholar
Green, H. J., & Hood, M. (2013). Significance of epistemological beliefs for teaching and learning psychology: A review. Psychology Learning & Teaching, 12(2), 168–178.
Article
Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005). Structure and strength in causal induction. Cognitive Psychology, 51(4), 334–384.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Große, C. S., & Renkl, A. (2007). Finding and fixing errors in worked examples: Can this foster learning outcomes? Learning and Instruction, 17(6), 612–634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.09.008
Article
Google Scholar
Grotzer, T. A., & Shane Tutwiler, M. (2014). Simplifying causal complexity: How interactions between modes of causal induction and information availability lead to heuristic-driven reasoning. Mind, Brain, and Education, 8(3), 97–114.
Article
Google Scholar
Haber, N., Smith, E. R., Moscoe, E., Andrews, K., Audy, R., Bell, W., et al. (2018). Causal language and strength of inference in academic and media articles shared in social media (CLAIMS): A systematic review. PLoS ONE, 13(5), e0196346. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196346
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Hall, S. S., & Seery, B. L. (2006). Behind the facts: Helping students evaluate media reports of psychological research. Teaching of Psychology, 33(2), 101–104. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328023top3302_4
Article
Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: Disposition, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53(4), 449–455. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.449
Article
Google Scholar
Hammond, E. C., & Horn, D. (1954). The relationship between human smoking habits and death rates: A follow-up study of 187,766 men. Journal of the American Medical Association, 155(15), 1316–1328. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1954.03690330020006
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hastie, R. (2015). Causal thinking in judgments. In G. Keren and G. Wu (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making, First Edition (pp. 590–628). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118468333.ch21
Hatfield, J., Faunce, G. J., & Soames Job, R. F. (2006). Avoiding confusion surrounding the phrase “correlation does not imply causation.” Teaching of Psychology, 33(1), 49–51.
Google Scholar
Hofer, B. K. (2000). Dimensionality and disciplinary differences in personal epistemology. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 378–405. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1026
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Horn, K. (2001). The consequences of citing hedged statements in scientific research articles. BioScience, 51(12), 1086–1093.
Article
Google Scholar
Huber, C. R., & Kuncel, N. R. (2015). Does college teach critical thinking? A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 20(10), 1–38. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654315605917
Article
Google Scholar
Huggins-Manley, A. C., Wright, E. A., Depue, K., & Oberheim, S. T. (2021). Unsupported causal inferences in the professional counseling literature base. Journal of Counseling and Development, 99(3), 243–251. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12371
Article
Google Scholar
Hyland, K. (1998). Boosting, hedging and the negotiation of academic knowledge. Text & Talk, 18(3), 349–382.
Google Scholar
Jenkins, E. W. (1994). Scientific literacy. In T. Husen & T. N. Postlethwaite (Eds.), The international encyclopedia of education (2nd ed., Vol. 9, pp. 5345–5350). Pergamon Press.
Google Scholar
Jensen, J. D. (2008). Scientific uncertainty in news coverage of cancer research: Effects of hedging on scientists’ and journalists’ credibility. Human Communication Research, 34, 347–369.
Article
Google Scholar
Johnson, H. M., & Seifert, C. M. (1994). Sources of the continued influence effect: When discredited information in memory affects later inferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(6), 1420–1436.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
Google Scholar
Kida, T. E. (2006). Don’t believe everything you think: The 6 basic mistakes we make in thinking. Prometheus Books.
Google Scholar
Klaczynski, P. A. (2000). Motivated scientific reasoning biases, epistemological beliefs, and theory polarization: A two-process approach to adolescent cognition. Child Development, 71(5), 1347–1366. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00232
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Koch, C., & Wüstemann, J. (2014). Experimental analysis. In The Oxford handbook of public accountability (pp. 127–142).
Koehler, J. J. (1993). The influence of prior beliefs on scientific judgments of evidence quality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 56, 28–55.
Article
Google Scholar
Kolstø, S. D., Bungum, B., Arnesen, E., Isnes, A., Kristensen, T., Mathiassen, K., & Ulvik, M. (2006). Science students’ critical examination of scientific information related to socio-scientific issues. Science Education, 90(4), 632–655. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20133
Article
Google Scholar
Kosonen, P., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Effects of teaching statistical laws on reasoning about everyday problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(1), 33. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.87.1.33
Article
Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. (1993). Connecting scientific and informal reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39(1), 74–103.
Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. (2005). Education for thinking. Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. (2012). The development of causal reasoning. Wires Cognitive Science, 3, 327–335. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1160
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., O’Loughlin, M., Schauble, L., Leadbeater, B., & Yotive, W. (1988). Developmental psychology series. The development of scientific thinking skills. Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Kuhn, D., & Dean, D., Jr. (2004). Connecting scientific reasoning and causal inference. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5(2), 261–288.
Article
Google Scholar
Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., & Andersen, C. (1995). Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60, i 157.
Google Scholar
Kuhn, D., Iordanou, K., Pease, M., & Wirkala, C. (2008). Beyond control of variables: What needs to develop to achieve skilled scientific thinking? Cognitive Development, 23, 435–451.
Article
Google Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lazarus, C., Haneef, R., Ravaud, P., & Boutron, I. (2015). Classification and prevalence of spin in abstracts of non-randomized studies evaluating an intervention. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 15, 85. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-015-0079-x
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Lee, L. O., James, P., Zevon, E. S., Kim, E. S., Trudel-Fitzgerald, C., Spiro, A., III., Grodstein, F., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2019). Optimism is associated with exceptional longevity in 2 epidemiologic cohorts of men and women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(37), 18357–18362. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1900712116
Article
Google Scholar
Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2006). Scientific thinking and science literacy. In W. Damon, & R. Lerner (Series Eds.) & K. A. Renninger, & I. E. Sigel (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology Vol. 4: Child psychology in practice (6th ed.). New York: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0405.
Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K., Seifert, C. M., Schwarz, N., & Cook, J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction: Continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 13(3), 106–131. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612451018
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Marinescu, I. E., Lawlor, P. N., & Kording, K. P. (2018). Quasi-experimental causality in neuroscience and behavioural research. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(12), 891–898.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E. (2005). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning, 41, 31–48.
Article
Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E. (2020). Multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Michal, A. L., Zhong, Y., & Shah, P. (2021). When and why do people act on flawed science? Effects of anecdotes and prior beliefs on evidence-based decision-making. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 6, 28. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-021-00293-2
Article
Google Scholar
Miller, J. D. (1996). Scientific literacy for effective citizenship: Science/technology/society as reform in science education. SUNY Press.
Google Scholar
Morling, B. (2014). Research methods in psychology: Evaluating a world of information. W.W. Norton and Company
Mueller, J. (2020). Correlation or causation? Retrieved June 1, 2021, from http://jfmueller.faculty.noctrl.edu/100/correlation_or_causation.htm
Mueller, J. F., & Coon, H. M. (2013). Undergraduates’ ability to recognize correlational and causal language before and after explicit instruction. Teaching of Psychology, 40(4), 288–293. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628313501038
Article
Google Scholar
Next Generation Science Standards Lead States. (2013). Next generation science standards: For states, by states. The National Academies Press.
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175
Article
Google Scholar
Norcross, J. C., Gerrity, D. M., & Hogan, E. M. (1993). Some outcomes and lessons from a cross-sectional evaluation of psychology undergraduates. Teaching of Psychology, 20(2), 93–96. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328023top2002_6
Article
Google Scholar
Norris, S. P., & Phillips, L. M. (1994). Interpreting pragmatic meaning when reading popular reports of science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31(9), 947–967. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660310909
Article
Google Scholar
Norris, S. P., Phillips, L. M., & Korpan, C. A. (2003). University students’ interpretation of media reports of science and its relationship to background knowledge, interest, and reading difficulty. Public Understanding of Science, 12(2), 123–145. https://doi.org/10.1177/09636625030122001
Article
Google Scholar
NTSA Framework (2012). Retrieved June 1, 2021 from https://ngss.nsta.org/practices.aspx?id=7
Ohlsson, S. (1996). Learning from error and the design of task environments. International Journal of Educational Research, 25(5), 419–448.
Article
Google Scholar
Pearl, J. (1995). Causal diagrams for empirical research. Biometrika, 82(4), 669–688.
Article
Google Scholar
Pearl, J. (2000). Causality: Models, reasoning, and inference. Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Pearl, J., & Mackenzie, D. (2018). The book of why: The new science of cause and effect. Basic Books.
Google Scholar
Picardi, C. A., & Masick, K. D. (2013). Research methods: Designing and conducting research with a real-world focus. SAGE Publications.
Google Scholar
Pressley, M., Wood, E., Woloshyn, V. E., Martin, V., King, A., & Menke, D. (1992). Encouraging mindful use of prior knowledge: Attempting to construct explanatory answers facilitates learning. Educational Psychologist, 27(1), 91–109.
Article
Google Scholar
Reinhart, A. L., Haring, S. H., Levin, J. R., Patall, E. A., & Robinson, D. H. (2013). Models of not-so-good behavior: Yet another way to squeeze causality and recommendations for practice out of correlational data. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 241–247.
Article
Google Scholar
Reis, H. T., & Judd, C. M. (2000). Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Renken, M. D., McMahan, E. A., & Nitkova, M. (2015). Initial validation of an instrument measuring psychology-specific epistemological beliefs. Teaching of Psychology, 42(2), 126–136.
Article
Google Scholar
Renkl, A., Stark, R., Gruber, H., & Mandl, H. (1998). Learning from worked-out examples: The effects of example variability and elicited self-explanations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23(1), 90–108. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1997.0959
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rhodes, R. E., Rodriguez, F., & Shah, P. (2014). Explaining the alluring influence of neuroscience information on scientific reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(5), 1432–1440. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036844
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rodriguez, F., Ng, A., & Shah, P. (2016a). Do college students notice errors in evidence when critically evaluating research findings? Journal on Excellence in College Teaching, 27(3), 63–78.
Google Scholar
Rodriguez, F., Rhodes, R. E., Miller, K., & Shah, P. (2016b). Examining the influence of anecdotal stories and the interplay of individual differences on reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 22(3), 274–296. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2016.1139506
Article
Google Scholar
Ryder, J. (2001). Identifying science understanding for functional scientific literacy. Studies in Science Education, 36(1), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057260108560166
Article
Google Scholar
Sá, W. C., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1999). The domain specificity and generality of belief bias: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 497–510. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.497
Article
Google Scholar
Schellenberg, E. G. (2020). Correlation = causation? Music training, psychology, and neuroscience. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14(4), 475–480.
Article
Google Scholar
Seifert, C. M., & Hutchins, E. L. (1992). Error as opportunity: Learning in a cooperative task. Human-Computer Interaction, 7(4), 409–435.
Article
Google Scholar
Shafto, P., Goodman, N. D., & Griffiths, T. L. (2014). A rational account of pedagogical reasoning: Teaching by, and learning from, examples. Cognitive Psychology, 71(1), 55–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.12.004
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shah, P., Michal, A., Ibrahim, A., Rhodes, R., & Rodriguez, F. (2017). What makes everyday scientific reasoning so challenging? The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 66, 251–299. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2016.11.006
Article
Google Scholar
Shou, Y., & Smithson, M. (2015). Effects of question formats on causal judgments and model evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 467. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00467.
Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (2008). Differentiation and integration: Guiding principles for analyzing cognitive change. Developmental Science, 11(4), 433–448. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00689.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sinatra, G. M., Kienhues, D., & Hofer, B. (2014). Addressing challenges to public understanding of science: Epistemic cognition, motivated reasoning, and conceptual change. Educational Psychologist, 49(2), 123–138. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2014.916216
Article
Google Scholar
Skelton, J. (1988). The care and maintenance of hedges. ELT Journal, 42(1), 37–43.
Article
Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (2005). Causal models. Oxford University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A., & Lagnado, D. A. (2003). Causal invariance in reasoning and learning. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 287–325.
Article
Google Scholar
Sloman, S., & Lagnado, D. A. (2015). Causality in thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 223–247.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Stadtler, M., Scharrer, L., Brummernhenrich, B., & Bromme, R. (2013). Dealing with uncertainty: Readers’ memory for and use of conflicting information from science texts as function of presentation format and source expertise. Cognition and Instruction, 31(2), 130–150. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2013.769996
Article
Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: The psychology of rational thought. Yale University.
Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2010). How to think straight about psychology (9th ed.). Allyn & Bacon.
Google Scholar
Stark, R., Kopp, V., & Fischer, M. R. (2011). Case-based learning with worked examples in complex domains: Two experimental studies in undergraduate medical education. Learning and Instruction, 21(1), 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.10.001
Article
Google Scholar
Stark, R., Mandl, H., Gruber, H., & Renkl, A. (2002). Conditions and effects of example elaboration. Learning and Instruction, 12(1), 39–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0959-4752(01)00015-9
Article
Google Scholar
Steffens, B., Britt, M. A., Braasch, J. L., Strømsø, H., & Bråten, I. (2014). Memory for scientific arguments and their sources: Claim–evidence consistency matters. Discourse Processes, 51, 117–142.
Article
Google Scholar
Sullivan, G. M. (2011). Getting off the “gold standard”: Randomized controlled trials and education research. Journal of Graduate Medical Education, 3(3), 285–289. https://doi.org/10.4300/JGME-D-11-00147.1
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Sumner, P., Vivian-Griffiths, S., Boivin, J., Williams, A., Venetis, C. A., Davies, A., Ogden, J., Whelan, L., Hughes, B., Dalton, B., Boy, F., & Chambers, C. D. (2014). The association between exaggeration in health related science news and academic press releases: Retrospective observational study. British Medical Journal, 2014(349), g7015. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.g7015
Article
Google Scholar
Tal, A., & Wansink, B. (2016). Blinded with science: Trivial graphs and formulas increase ad persuasiveness and belief in product efficacy. Public Understanding of Science, 25(1), 117–125. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662514549688
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Topor, D. D. (2019). If you’re happy and you know it… you may live longer. Harvard Health Blog, Harvard Medical School (10.16.2019). Retrieved June 1, 2021, from https://www.health.harvard.edu/blog/if-you-are-happy-and-you-know-it-you-may-live-longer-2019101618020
Trefil, J. (2008). Science education for everyone: Why and what? Liberal Education, 94(2), 6–11. Retrieved 6/15/2021 from: https://www.aacu.org/publications-research/periodicals/science-education-everyone-why-and-what
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1977). Causal thinking in judgment under uncertainty. In J. Hintikka & R. E. Butts (Eds.), Basic problems in methodology and linguistics (pp. 167–190). Springer.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., & Rummel, N. (2010). Example-based learning: Integrating cognitive and social-cognitive research perspectives. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 155–174. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9134-7
Article
Google Scholar
Waldmann, M. R., Hagmayer, Y., & Blaisdell, A. P. (2006). Beyond the information given: Causal models in learning and reasoning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 307–311. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00458.x
Article
Google Scholar
Whoriskey, P. (2011). Requiring algebra 2 in high school gains momentum. The Washington Post. Retrieved 6/15/2021 from https://www.washingtonpost.com/business/economy/requiring_algebra_ii_in_high_school_gains_momentum_nationwide/2011/04/01/AF7FBWXC_story.html?noredirect=on&utm_term=.a153d444a4bd
Wright, J. C., & Murphy, G. L. (1984). The utility of theories in intuitive statistics: The robustness of theory-based judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 301–322.
Article
Google Scholar
Xiong, C., Shapiro, J., Hullman, J., & Franconeri, S. (2020). Illusion of causality in visualized data. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 26(1), 853–862. https://doi.org/10.1109/TVCG.2019.2934399
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Yavchitz, A., Boutron, I., Bafeta, A., Marroun, I., Charles, P., Mantz, J., & Ravaud, P. (2012). Misrepresentation of randomized controlled trials in press releases and news coverage: A cohort study. PLoS Medicine, 9, e1001308.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Zimmerman, C., Bisanz, G. L., Bisanz, J., Klein, J. S., & Klein, P. (2001). Science at the supermarket: A comparison of what appears in the popular press, experts’ advice to readers, and what students want to know. Public Understanding of Science, 10(1), 37–58.
Article
Google Scholar
Zweig, M., & Devoto, E. (2015). Observational studies—Does the language fit the evidence? Association versus causation. Health News Review. Retrieved 6/15/2021 from https://www.healthnewsreview.org/toolkit/tips-for-understanding-studies/does-the-language-fit-the-evidence-association-versus-causation/