Allington, D., Duffy, B., Wessely, S., Dhavan, N., & Rubin, J. (2020). Health-protective behaviour, social media usage and conspiracy belief during the COVID-19 public health emergency. Psychological Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172000224X.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bago, B., Rand, D. G., & Pennycook, G. (2020). Fake news, fast and slow: Deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news headlines. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000729.
Article
Google Scholar
Barron, D., Furnham, A., Weis, L., Morgan, K. D., Towell, T., & Swami, V. (2018). The relationship between schizotypal facets and conspiracist beliefs via cognitive processes. Psychiatry Research, 259, 15–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.10.001.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.
Article
Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., Lilienfeld, S. O., Rowan, K. A., Masciocchi, C. M., & Grain, F. (2020). The generality of belief in unsubstantiated claims. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34, 16–28. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3581.
Article
Google Scholar
Brewer, S. (1998). Scientific expert testimony and intellectual due process. The Yale Law Journal, 107(6), 1535–1681. https://doi.org/10.2307/797336.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bronstein, M. V., Pennycook, G., Bear, A., Rand, D. G., & Cannon, T. D. (2019). Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(1), 108–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.09.005.
Article
Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: the life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 197. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.197.
Article
Google Scholar
Derwin, A. C. C. (2018). The judicial admission of faulty scientific expert evidence informing wrongful convictions. Western Journal of Legal Studies, 8(2), 1–19.
Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M., Cichocka, A., Nefes, T., Ang, C. S., & Deravi, F. (2019). Understanding conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 40, 3–35. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12568.
Article
Google Scholar
Drinkwater, K., Denovan, A., Dagnall, N., & Parker, A. (2017). An assessment of the dimensionality and factorial structure of the revised paranormal belief scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1693. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01693.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Dror, I. E. (2016). A hierarchy of expert performance. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(2), 121–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.03.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Edmond, G. (2015). Forensic science evidence and the conditions for rational (jury) evaluation. Melbourne University Law Review, 39(1), 77–127.
Google Scholar
Edmond, G., Found, B., Martire, K., Ballantyne, K., Hamer, D., Searston, R., et al. (2016). Model forensic science. Australian Journal of Forensic Sciences, 48(5), 496–537. https://doi.org/10.1080/00450618.2015.1128969.
Article
Google Scholar
Eleventh circuit pattern jury instructions, criminal. (2020). Atlanta, GA.
Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Frau-Meigs, D. (2019). Societal costs of “fake news” in the Digital Single Market. European Parliament.
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1257/089533005775196732.
Article
Google Scholar
Garrett, B. L. (2017). Actual innocence and wrongful convictions. Academy for Justice, A Report on Scholarship and Criminal Justice Reform (Erik Luna ed., 2017 Forthcoming).
Garrett, B. L., & Mitchell, G. (2018). The proficiency of experts. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 166(4), 901–960. https://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.2402.
Article
Google Scholar
Garrett, B. L., & Neufeld, P. J. (2009). Invalid forensic science testimony and wrongful convictions. Virginia Law Review, 95(1), 1–97.
Google Scholar
Giannelli, P. C. (1993). Junk science: The criminal cases. The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 84(1), 105.
Article
Google Scholar
Greene, C. M., & Murphy, G. (this issue). Individual differences in susceptibility to false memories for COVID-19 fake news. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications
Gross, S. R. (1991). Expert evidence. Wisconsin Law Review, 1113–1232. https://repository.law.umich.edu/articles/196
Hand, L. (1901). Historical and practical considerations regarding expert testimony. Harvard Law Review, 15(1), 40–58. https://doi.org/10.2307/1322532.
Article
Google Scholar
Hilbert, J. (2019). The disappointing history of science in the courtroom: Frye, Daubert, and the ongoing crisis of junk science in criminal trials. Oklahoma Law Review, 71(3), 759–822.
Google Scholar
Hills, T. T. (2019). The dark side of information proliferation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(3), 323–330. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618803647.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2020). A bioweapon or a hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ye3ma.
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS ONE, 9(2), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177.
Article
Google Scholar
Judicial Commission of New South Wales, issuing body. (2020). Criminal trial courts bench book Retrieved October 13, 2020, from http://nla.gov.au/nla.obj-467012383.
Jurs, A. W. (2015). Expert prevalence, persuasion, and price: What trial participants really think about experts? Indiana Law Journal, 91, 353–391.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Macmillan.
Google Scholar
Kleiman, E. (2017). EMAtools: Data management tools for real-time monitoring/ecological momentary assessment data. R package version 0.1. 3.
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26.
Article
Google Scholar
Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K., & Cook, J. (2017). Beyond misinformation: Understanding and coping with the “post-truth” era. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(4), 353–369.
Article
Google Scholar
Lindeman, M., & Aarnio, K. (2007). Superstitious, magical, and paranormal beliefs: An integrative model. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(4), 731–744.
Article
Google Scholar
Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 617–625. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3042.
Article
Google Scholar
Martire, K. A., & Edmond, G. (2016). Rethinking expert opinion evidence. Melbourne University Law Review, 40, 967.
Google Scholar
Martire, K. A., Edmond, G., & Navarro, D. (2020). Exploring juror evaluations of expert opinions using the Expert Persuasion Expectancy framework. Legal and Criminological Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1111/lcrp.12165.
Article
Google Scholar
McAuliff, B. D., & Duckworth, T. D. (2010). I spy with my little eye: Jurors’ detection of internal validity threats in expert evidence. Law and Human Behavior, 34(6), 489–500. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-010-9219-3.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
McAuliff, B. D., Kovera, M. B., & Nunez, G. (2009). Can jurors recognize missing control groups, confounds, and experimenter bias in psychological science? Law and Human Behavior, 33(3), 247–257. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9133-0.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mikušková, E. B. (2018). Conspiracy beliefs of future teachers. Current Psychology, 37(3), 692–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9561-4.
Article
Google Scholar
Mnookin, J. L. (2007). Expert evidence, partisanship, and epistemic competence. Brooklyn Law Review, 73, 1009–1033.
Google Scholar
National Research Council of the Academies of Science. (2009). Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A path forward. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/0379-0738(86)90074-5.
Book
Google Scholar
Norris, P., & Epstein, S. (2011). An experiential thinking style: its facets and relations with objective and subjective criterion measures. Journal of Personality, 79(5), 1043–1080. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00718.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Oldt, R. F., & Kanthaswamy, S. (2020). Expanded CODIS STR allele frequencies—Evidence for the irrelevance of race-based DNA databases. Legal Medicine, 42, 101642. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.legalmed.2019.101642.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). On the reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshit. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(6), 549–563.
Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2015). Everyday consequences of analytic thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(6), 425–432. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2644392.
Article
Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2019). Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition, 188, 39–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.011.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Who falls for fake news? The roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity, and analytic thinking. Journal of Personality, 88(2), 185–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12476.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
President's Council of Advisors on Science and Technology. (2016). Forensic science in criminal courts: ensuring scientific validity of feature-comparison methods. United States: Executive Office of the President's Council of Advisors on Science and Technology.
Raeder, M. (2003). What does innocence have to do with it: Commentary on wrongful convictions and rationality? Law Review of Michigan State University Detroit College of Law, 2003(4), 1315–1336. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316417119.010.
Article
Google Scholar
Rizeq, J., Flora, D. B., & Toplak, M. E. (2020). An examination of the underlying dimensional structure of three domains of contaminated mindware: Paranormal beliefs, conspiracy beliefs, and anti-science attitudes. Thinking & Reasoning. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1759688.
Article
Google Scholar
Ross, R. M., Pennycook, G., McKay, R., Gervais, W. M., Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2016). Analytic cognitive style, not delusional ideation, predicts data gathering in a large beads task study. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 21(4), 300–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2016.1192025.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Schweitzer, N. J., & Saks, M. J. (2009). The gatekeeper effect: The impact of judges’ admissibility decisions on the persuasiveness of expert testimony. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 15(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015290.
Article
Google Scholar
Sindermann, C., Cooper, A., & Montag, C. (2020). A short review on susceptibility to falling for fake political news. Current Opinion in Psychology, 36, 44–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.03.014.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ståhl, T., & Van Prooijen, J. W. (2018). Epistemic rationality: Skepticism toward unfounded beliefs requires sufficient cognitive ability and motivation to be rational. Personality and Individual Differences, 122, 155–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.026.
Article
Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(2), 342–357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.2.342.
Article
Google Scholar
Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered questions: A preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11 conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749–761. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1583.
Article
Google Scholar
Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133(3), 572–585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Thayer, J. (1890). “Law and fact” in jury trials. Harvard Law Review, 4(4), 147–175. https://doi.org/10.2307/1321285.
Article
Google Scholar
Thompson, W. C., & Schumann, E. L. (1987). Interpretation of statistical evidence in criminal trials. Law and Human Behavior, 11, 167–187. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01044641.
Article
Google Scholar
van der Linden, S. (2015). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 171–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.045.
Article
Google Scholar
von Ahn, L., Maurer, B., McMillen, C., Abraham, D., & Blum, M. (2008). ReCAPTCHA: Human-based character recognition via web security measures. Science, 321(5895), 1465–1468.
Article
Google Scholar
Ward, T. (2017). Expert testimony, law and epistemic authority. Journal of Applied Philosophy, 34(2), 263–277. https://doi.org/10.1111/japp.12213.
Article
Google Scholar
Ward, T., & Garety, P. A. (2017). Fast and slow thinking in distressing delusions: A review of the literature and implications for targeted therapy. Schizophrenia Research, 203, 80–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.08.045.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Zhang, L.-F., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The nature of intellectual styles. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Google Scholar
Zimmermann, F., & Kohring, M. (2020). Mistrust, disinforming news, and vote choice: A panel survey on the origins and consequences of believing disinformation in the 2017 German Parliamentary Election. Political Communication, 37(2), 215–237. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2019.1686095.
Article
Google Scholar