Skip to main content

Visual communication via the design of food and beverage packaging

Abstract

A rapidly growing body of empirical research has recently started to emerge highlighting the connotative and/or semiotic meanings that consumers typically associate with specific abstract visual design features, such as colours (either when presented individually or in combination), simple shapes/curvilinearity, and the orientation and relative position of those design elements on product packaging. While certain of our affective responses to such basic visual design features appear almost innate, the majority are likely established via the internalization of the statistical regularities of the food and beverage marketplace (i.e. as a result of associative learning), as in the case of round typeface and sweet-tasting products. Researchers continue to document the wide range of crossmodal correspondences that underpin the links between individual visual packaging design features and specific properties of food and drink products (such as their taste, flavour, or healthfulness), and the ways in which marketers are now capitalizing on such understanding to increase sales. This narrative review highlights the further research that is still needed to establish the connotative or symbolic/semiotic meaning(s) of particular combinations of design features (such as coloured stripes in a specific orientation), as opposed to individual cues in national food markets and also, increasingly, cross-culturally in the case of international brands.

Introduction

The visual design of food and beverage product packaging is at something of a crossroads. The field currently lies between the traditional art and design approach—often based on the intuitions of creative designers/marketers (and/or the results of focus groups or in-depth interviews; Cheskin, 1957, 1967, 1972; Lunt, 1981; Rapaille, 2007; Stern, 1981)—and the more scientific approach to visual communication (i.e. presenting information graphically, such that it creates meaning concerning the product and its attributes/brand associations; Underwood, 1993, 1999; Underwood & Klein, 2002; Underwood & Ozanne, 1998; Underwood et al., 2001). The latter approach is increasingly coming to be based on our growing understanding of, for example, the crossmodal correspondences (Spence, 2011, 2012; Velasco & Spence, 2019a; Velasco et al., 2016b; cf. Batra et al., 2016; Schifferstein et al., 2013; Skaczkowski et al., 2016; Thomson, 2016).

Crossmodal correspondences refer to the tendency for a feature or attribute in one sensory modality (e.g. the colours pink and red) to be associated with a sensory feature in another sensory modality (e.g. a sweet taste; Ngo et al., 2013; Spence & Parise, 2012; Woods et al., 2013). Often, these connections between the senses are surprising, much like synaesthesia.Footnote 1 Indeed, some researchers have even suggested that synaesthetic inducer-concurrent relations could be used productively in the field of product design (Haverkamp, 2014) and/or product packaging/marketing (cf. Crisinel & Spence, 2012). That said, it is important to stress that the approach outlined here, based on crossmodal correspondences, differs from the phenomenon of synaesthesia in that the cross-sensory connections expressed in the former case tend to be shared between people, whereas synaesthesia is defined by the idiosyncratic nature of the inducer-concurrent mapping (see Deroy & Spence, 2013; Spence, 2019).

Visual design features are not only associated with taste/flavour attributes,Footnote 2 but with a range of connotative and semantic meanings (e.g. green = healthy) as typically assessed by research using the semantic differential technique (e.g. Morich, 1981; Snider & Osgood, 1969; see also Kunz et al., 2020). However, design cues (such as colour) are also used to set consumer expectations around product variant, brand, quality, and price (with black packaging often linked with luxury and premiumness, whereas orange is typically associated with cheapness; see Velasco & Spence, 2019c; Wheatley, 1973). Given that we typically see colour in context (Elliot & Maier, 2012), and that context is (at times) influenced by culture, it might be thought that it would be unlikely for there to be many universal meanings associated with specific visual design features, such as a particular hue. That said, Tham et al. (2020) recently tested English monolinguals, Chinese bilinguals, and Chinese monolinguals in order to establish the conceptual associations that the different groups had with colour words and colour patches. According to their results, white was associated with purity, blue was related to water/sky themes, green was linked to healthy, purple was regal, and pink was linked to female for all three groups. At the same time, however, red and orange were associated with enthusiastic in Chinese, whereas red was associated with attraction in English. In other words, Tham et al.’s results highlight the existence of both a number of cross-cultural similarities and differences in the conceptual associations that different groups of people appear to hold with colours and colour words.

In this narrative review, and in relation to visual design, we are particularly interested in the crossmodal correspondences that may exist between various ‘abstract’ visual featuresFootnote 3—colours (either when presented individually or in combination), simple shapes/curvilinearity, and the orientation and relative position of those design elements on the packaging—and the chemical senses (specifically taste/flavour). That said, several other connotative/symbolic/semantics associations of visual features/attributes (e.g. with healthy/natural, price, premiumness, etc.) will also be discussed (see Marques da Rosa et al., 2019). It is important to stress here that the term ‘abstract’ here refers to those features that are not associated with a specific object—while many abstract visual design features can be classed as simple stimuli, some patterns and face-like arrangements of lines might be considered complex. Hence, a patch of blue or a specific simple shape like a circle can be considered abstract design features, whereas the picture or outline of a hamburger, say, or the image of some fruit (see Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2013), would not.

Visual design of product packaging based on crossmodal correspondences

While a handful of famous designers and marketers have long been lauded for their design choices that helped boost long-term brand/product success (see Cheskin, 1957, 1967, 1972; Dichter, 1975; Favre & November, 1979; Graham, 2016), it often appeared as though their decisions were based on intuition, sometimes backed-up by the results of consumer/focus-group research and in-depth interviews (Catterall & Maclaran, 2006; Lunt, 1981; Samuel, 2010). By contrast, an emerging body of empirical research on the crossmodal correspondences is now starting to help establish the connotative meaning of a variety of different abstract visual design features. In particular, a broad array of findings from experimental psychology have helped to establish the meanings (connotative and otherwise) that are associated with (or primed by) everything from colours (Déribéré, 1978; Ho et al., 2014; Spence, 2020a; Van Doorn et al., 2014) to shapes (Dichter, 1971; Mirabito et al., 2017; Motoki & Velasco, 2021; Spence, 2012, 2020b; Spence & Van Doorn, 2017; Van Doorn et al., 2017; Velasco & Spence, 2019a; see also Yarar et al., 2019), and from curvilinearity to the relative position of various design elements on product packaging (Romero & Biswas, 2016; Simmonds et al., 2018b; Sundar & Noseworthy, 2014; Velasco et al., 2015c).

Several of these visual cues, such as a curved line that, at least when presented horizontally, can be interpreted as a smile (Karim et al., 2017; Salgado-Montejo et al., 2015b; cf. Kühn et al., 2014; Windhager et al., 2008) and patterns that may be interpreted as looking like a snake, spider, or scorpion (Hoehl et al., 2017; Isbell, 2006; Van Lee et al., 2013; LoBue, 2014; Spence, 2021a) have been associated with possibly innate responses that are (often) attention-capturing, albeit typically negatively valenced in the latter cases. However, the meaning of many other visual design cues is much more likely to be established on the basis of associative learning.Footnote 4 Here it is also important to consider the commonly accepted symbolic and semiotic meaning of packaging design features in the food and beverage marketplace (e.g. cartoon portrait logos and their association, in Western cultures, with their often humorous messages; Barthel, 1989; Danesi, 2013; Garber et al., 2008; Gardner & Levy, 1955; Levy, 1959; Mick, 1986; Plasschaert, 1995).

Researchers have, for example, highlighted how the packaging of flavours/varieties of crisps/potato chips tend to be coloured in a specific (albeit somewhat arbitrary) manner (see Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2012). Green packaging, for example, is often (though not always) associated with cheese and onion flavour in the UK, whereas blue packaging typically signals salt and vinegar instead (though see Visser, 2009, p. 109, on the suggestion that salt and vinegar type crisps are colour-coded purple-pink). As another example, one might take the different colour associations with full-, half-, and low-fat milk that exist in different parts of the world (Simmonds & Spence, 2019). In Australia, for example, light milk (i.e. 2% milk) is often tagged with colour-coded caps that are light blue, while full-cream milk often has dark blue caps and labels (Cutolo, 2021). By contrast, in the UK, skimmed milk (containing less than 0.3% fat) is typically tagged with red caps, semi-skimmed milk (i.e. less than 2% fat) is tagged with green, and full-fat milk is tagged with the colour blue.

Different packaging label colours are sometimes also associated with different forms of animal protein (e.g. consider the different colour codes that are often used to help distinguish lamb, beef, poultry, and pork) in the fresh meat category (see Simmonds & Spence, 2019, for a review). As such, a particular hue may be associated with a range of different attributes/qualities, and the extent to which different associations are primed may well ultimately depend on the context in which that colour happens to be presented (see Elliot & Maier, 2012; Motoki & Velasco, 2021), and the familiarity of the consumer with the conventions of the marketplace in which they happen to find themselves.

Such market-/country-specific differences also speak to the role of culture (built on habit and prior experience/exposure) in determining the meaning of colour in a given context (see also Jonauskaite et al., 2019a). It is important to stress, though, that in contrast to the often-published observations of those in marketing who, over the years, have attempted to map out the abstract meaning of colours (e.g. Aslam, 2006; Jacobs et al., 1991; Wheatley, 1973), or emotional associations with colours (e.g. Adams & Osgood, 1973), colour is nearly always seen in context (Elliot & Maier, 2012; though see also Amsteus et al., 2015). Furthermore, researchers have recently argued for the importance of context in terms of a theory of semantic discriminability (Mukherjee et al., 2022; Schloss et al., 2021). According to the latter researchers, the mapping of a colour to a particular concept is often inferred on the basis of other stimuli in the comparison group, rather than being based directly on the strength of the underlying association.Footnote 5 Figure 1 frames Mukherjee and colleagues’ distinction in the context of the colour of potato chip packaging. What their theory means, in practice, is that sometimes the inferred colour–flavour mapping need not necessarily reflect the strongest colour–flavour association.

Fig. 1
figure 1

adapted from Schloss et al. (2018)

Distinction between colour–flavour associations and inferred mappings, showing colour–flavour association strengths for flavours ‘Cheese and onion’ and ‘Salt and vinegar’ with crisp packaging colours blue, green, and pinkish-purple (thicker lines connecting flavours with colours indicate stronger associations). What such a hypothetical situation highlights is how the colour–flavour mapping may result from inference rather than direct association. Figure

Various abstract visual design features normally exist on product packaging alongside semantic information concerning brand name, product description, (any) product imagery, and/or possibly also serving suggestions (Rebollar et al., 2012; Simmonds & Spence, 2019; Thomson, 2016; Visser, 2009). Although product, and other kinds of, visual imagery seen on packaging (or seen through transparent windows in the packaging) undoubtedly play an important role in determining the consumer’s impressions of a variety of food and beverage products (Simmonds & Spence, 2017, 2019; Simmonds et al., 2018a), reviewing the literature documenting the role played by such concrete (typically semantically meaningful) visual cues falls beyond the scope of this targeted narrative review. This review will instead focus specifically on abstract visual design features and their relation to product taste/flavour, healthiness, price, etc. Those readers interested in the influence of product imagery are directed to Simmonds and Spence’s (2019) review.

Review outline

In ‘On the meaning associated with individual abstract visual packaging cues’ section, we review what is currently known about the connotative meanings (including crossmodal correspondences) associated with specific design features such as colour, shape, orientation/position, and the use of convention-defying visual designs. In ‘Combining abstract visual design features’ section, the discussion is extended to the meaning of combinations of abstract visual cues using, as a recent commercial example, the under-researched combination of colour and stripes (i.e. a band of colour that differs from the colour on either side of it). Applied researchers have now deconstructed a number of elements of food and beverage packaging design in order to try and discern how to optimize everything from the connotation of ‘healthiness’ (Cavallo & Piqueras-Fiszman, 2017; Huang & Lu, 2013, 2015; Marques da Rosa et al., 2019; Reinoso-Carvalho et al., 2021), spiciness (Gil-Pérez et al., 2019), and quality (Pombo & Velasco, 2021; Wang, 2013). To date, however, only limited research has investigated the influence of vertical or horizontal orientation on consumer perception and product sales. This is demonstrated by the fact that in recent books on packaging (e.g. Velasco & Spence, 2019a, b), there is virtually no mention of the topic. As such, there is a need to review the existing literature and make recommendations for future research. The ‘Conclusions and future directions’ section offers some directions for future research. Areas that are not covered by this review include the consumers’ response to innovations in specific packaging design/technology, nor will issues related to the sustainability of product packaging be discussed (e.g. Associated Press, 2013; Azzi et al., 2012; Rundh, 2005; Silayoi & Speece, 2007).

Note that in addition to providing an up-to-date review of the literature on visual aspects of packaging design, we also highlight several further concrete areas for future packaging research. These include determining which of the many meanings associated with specific abstract design features such as colours or packaging shapes are primed in the mind of the consumer under everyday conditions (i.e. away from the specific task constraints typically imposed by the experimenter in most laboratory research). Having determined several different meanings that are associated, individually, with specific visual design features, further research is clearly also needed to help determine which cues dominate and/or how different abstract design features combine to convey specific meanings to consumers in different markets/contexts (Visser, 2009). A priori, one might consider whether sub-/super-/additive interactions will be observed when various visual design cues (e.g. colour and shape) are combined. Alternatively, however, it would also seem possible that one cue, such as colour might tend to dominate over other cues (such as, for example, colour dominating over shape, typeface, or texture). At the same time, however, it is also important to stress the fact that the intramodal perceptual grouping (Wagemans, 2015) of visual cues may give rise to a different meaning/association entirely than that associated with, or primed by, the individual sensory cues (see Dreksler & Spence, 2019).

Over the years, a number of different theoretical accounts have been put forward in order to try and explain the meanings/associations that may be primed by different visual design features (see Table 1 for a summary of the various accounts that have been used to help explain the meaning of abstract visual design cues). The accounts include (a) grounded cognition theory where, for example, a ‘strong is heavy’ metaphor is activated, and thus congruency dictates that heavy objects should appear at the bottom of packaging (Fenko et al., 2018), (b) conceptual metaphor theory where healthy foods are associated with high verticality, and thus should be situated at the top of product packaging (Wang & Basso, 2021), (c) the connotative meaning account based on the semantic differential technique (see Table 2 for a summary of the various different methods used by researchers in this area), and crossmodal correspondences (e.g. green = healthy; Morich, 1981), (d) the theory of semiotics where signs convey meaning (e.g. cartoon portrait logos mentioned above; Barthes, 1977; Chandler, 2017), and (e) various evolutionary explanations where stripes may have evolved to attract attention. Ultimately, in terms of parsimony, it would obviously be desirable to consider whether any unifying explanatory account might be invoked/developed to help provide an overarching explanation for the meaning of visual design. However, when we take a careful look at each visual design cue in turn (see below), there is as yet little progress in developing such a commonly agreed account of visual design.

Table 1 Summary of various meanings of abstract visual design cues in product packaging
Table 2 Methods used to assess the nature and/or strength of the expectations and associations that people/consumers hold with specific visual features in product packaging

As highlighted in Fig. 2, it is clear that there are multiple roles for visual design cues, both related to communicating meaning, or setting expectations, as well as in terms of attentional capture in a realistic visual (multisensory) environment (e.g. Peng-Li et al., 2020). Certainly, there is interest in those factors that facilitate attentional selection (Reutskaja et al., 2011). Here, it is worth stressing that visual design of product packaging has not only been shown to set specific expectations but can also modify people’s product experience. Very often, the approach used by researchers in this area is first to establish the expectations that are primed in the mind of the consumer on being presented with specific packaging designs. Thereafter, on occasion, researchers will then assess whether the differing expectations set by different packaging designs carry through to influence the consumer’s experience of the product itself (de Sousa et al., 2020; Togawa et al., 2019; Van Rompay et al., 2019; cf. Carvalho & Spence, 2019).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Assessment of visual design choices regarding specific individual design features (e.g. use of a particular colour or shape) at various stages of the packaging (design) journey

Ultimately, of course, the role of effective product packaging is not solely to communicate with the consumer and, on occasion, to enhance product experience, packaging also plays an important role in capturing the consumer’s attention on the shelf or online product display (see Fig. 2). It is intriguing to note here how a distinct body of research has attempted to assess the effectiveness of attentional capture, and the ease of finding a given target product on a more or less realistic shelf/online display (Reutskaja et al., 2011; Zhao et al., 2017). Ultimately, of course, the success of packaging designs is reflected in long-term sales, though here there simply tends to be less publically available research (Sugermeyer, 2021; cf. Kroese et al., 2016; Kühn et al., 2016).

On the meaning associated with individual abstract visual packaging cues

In this section, we review the evidence concerning the various meanings that may be associated with specific abstract visual cues in the context of product packaging (focusing primarily on the case of food and beverage packaging). Here, the focus will be on the meanings that consumers associate with colours, basic shapes, visual textures (Barbosa Escobar et al., 2020; see also Matthews et al., 2019), as well as the orientation and relative position of specific design elements. At the outset, it is worth highlighting the fact that there are different denotative, connotative, semiotic, and semantic meanings potentially associated with specific abstract visual design features, either when presented individually, or more commonly, when presented in combination (see Visser, 2009). That is, abstract visual design features may be associated with a specific product, brand, or category of product (see Baxter et al., 2018). Abstract visual design features such as colour or shape may also come to be associated with other product attributes such as healthiness, naturalness, indulgence, luxury, or cheapness (see Cavallo & Piqueras-Fiszman, 2017; Mai et al., 2016; Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2012; Schuldt, 2013; Tijssen et al., 2017; Velasco & Spence, 2019c, for examples). There is also a ‘green/environmental concern’ association with unsurprisingly, the colour green (see Schloss et al., 2018, in the context of recycling).

The focus in this review will primarily be on trying to understand the ‘meaning’ of various different abstract visual design features in terms of the crossmodal correspondences that have been established with sensory properties of the food and beverage products themselves, such as sweetness. At the same time, however, we will also summarize the relevant literature on the connotative meanings of abstract visual design features, such as active–passive, good-bad, dominant-submissive, that have been established by research using the semantic differential technique (Adams & Osgood, 1973; Henson et al., 2006; Osgood et al., 1957). Over the years, Word Association (Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2013), Implicit Association Tests (Parise & Spence, 2012), and Conjoint Analysis (Ares & Deliza, 2010; Baptista et al., submitted; Gislason et al., 2020), as well as focus group research (Lunt, 1981; Rapaille, 2007; Stern, 1981) have all been used by those researchers wanting to establish the more abstract, symbolic/semiotic meanings that may be associated with specific abstract visual design features (typically when embedded in product packaging) (see Table 2 for a summary of techniques). We presumably also need to consider the benefits of the consumer neuroscience, or neuromarketing approaches to design (see also Huang et al., 2021). However, it should be noted that despite a longstanding interest in the consumer neuroscience of product packaging (see Weinstein, 1981, for early research), the body of research that has been published to date remains fairly limited (see Moya et al., 2020, for a review).

Having set the background for our consideration of the various meanings associated with abstract visual packaging design cues in the world of food and beverage packaging, we will now take a closer look at each of the main visual design features in turn, starting with perhaps the most frequently studied abstract visual design feature, namely colour.

On the multiple meanings of packaging colour and other visual appearance cues

Perhaps the single most extensively studied visual design feature on product packaging is colour (Baptista et al., 2021; Crilly et al., 2004; Danger, 1968, 1987; Déribéré, 1978; Favre, 1968; Huang & Lu, 2013; Kovač et al., 2019; Labrecque & Milne, 2012, 2013; Labrecque et al., 2013; Merlo et al., 2018; Theben et al., 2020; Wheatley, 1973; see Spence & Velasco, 2018, for a review). Consider here only Coca-Cola’s dominant use of (and association with) the colour red (and rounded white text) which has been successfully linked to the brand and, by doing so, has seemingly managed to overcome any potential language/cultural barriers (Van Den Berg-Weitzel & Van Den Laar, 2001). The colour red and round typeface both also convey/prime notions of sweetness (Velasco et al., 2015b; Velasco et al., 2018a, b; Woods et al., 2016). However, in certain contexts red also acts as an indicator of temperature (i.e. warmth, think about the colour on taps; Ho et al., 2014, see Spence, 2020b, for a review) and can signal danger/prime avoidance motivation (Lunardo et al., 2021; cf. Labrecque & Milne, 2012), as well as attraction (Tham et al., 2020). In other words, a particular hue of product packaging may be associated with a range of attributes/qualities, and the extent to which any one of these different associations are primed may well depend on the context, or category, in which that colour is presented (Amsteus et al., 2015). Intriguingly, Coca-Cola’s main international competitor (Pepsi) rebranded some years ago, choosing the colour blue (Cooper, 1996), presumably to help distinguish itself within the cola beverage category (see also Baxter et al., 2018, on the importance of brand colour).

For further evidence of the learning of arbitrary associations between packaging colour scheme and flavour consider only the crisps/potato chips category, mentioned earlier (Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2012). That said, there appears to be some degree of consistency with which different colours are used to signal different flavour variants. For instance, Velasco et al. (2015a) demonstrated that congruency (e.g. red/tomato), relative to incongruency (e.g. yellow/tomato), between the colours used in product packaging and flavour labels facilitated their participants’ visual search performance (as evidenced by reduced reaction times) for target crisp packets. Packaging colour is, then, sometimes used to signal variation within a category, whereas, at other times, it may be associated with a particular brand (and thus indirectly also with a category instead).

Occasionally, however, brands have deliberately chosen to contravene the colour code of the category. Take, for example, the use of blue packaging for cheese-and-onion flavour crisps, and green packaging for salt-and-vinegar, introduced by Walkers in the UK to try to secure exposure of customers to their new flavour variety (c. 1984; see Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2012). This decision was apparently based on the notion that our shopping choices are, in large part, based on colour (see Spence & Velasco, 2018, for a review). Other crisp manufacturers in the UK had historically tagged salt-and-vinegar with blue. So, by packaging their new flavour variant (cheese and onion) in the well-establish blue of salt and vinegar, the idea was that consumers would shop by colour and hence be inadvertently exposed to a new flavour variant. Spence and Piqueras-Fiszman (2012) highlighted the example of a white wine that was called ‘Red’ and which had a bright red label, as an ultimately unsuccessful example of incongruency. Hence, sometimes abstract visual design features such as hue are chosen after considering both their ability to differentiate the product from others in the marketplace and the specific connotative meaning of the hue. The reader is referred to Labrecque and Milne (2013) for further discussion of colour norms and the benefits of colour differentiation in the marketplace (see Spence & Velasco, 2018; Vermeir & Roose, 2020, for reviews).

In addition to pink and red being associated with sweetness, Woods et al. (2016) demonstrated that white and blue were associated with saltiness, green and yellow with sourness, and black and green with bitterness. Consumers have also been shown to perceive a candy bar with a green label as being healthier than one with a red label, even when the caloric information on the labels happens to be identical (Schuldt, 2013). While the majority of the research that has been published to date has tended to focus on the colour of outer packaging, it is interesting to note that inner packaging colour has started to attract the attention of researchers, especially for those products such as individual yoghurt pots, where the consumer often consumes the product directly from the packaging (see van Esch et al., 2019; see also Krishna et al., 2017, on the importance of distinguishing between inner and outer packaging).

Taken together, the research that has been published to date highlights the multiple meanings that may be associated with a given colour in the context of food and beverage packaging. Given that packaging colour may be associated with one of a number of attributes including flavour (Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2012), variant (Cutolo, 2021), brand (as in the case of signature colours; Baxter et al., 2018), but also more generally with other attributes such as healthfulness (Mai et al., 2016; Schuldt, 2013; Tijssen et al., 2017; see also Cavallo & Piqueras-Fiszman, 2017) and luxury/cheapness (see Velasco & Spence, 2019c; Wheatley, 1973; see also Hagtvedt, 2014; Huang & Lu, 2013; Spence & Velasco, 2019, for other examples), the relevant question becomes: Which of the many possible meanings dominates in the mind of the consumer in any given situation or context? It is worth noting that a problem with much of the laboratory/online research conducted to date is that the dimension of interest to researchers has often been presented to consumers in the response scale’s anchor labels. This is obviously unlike the conditions of everyday life, where the most salient dimension of meaning might well be determined by the aisle in a supermarket, or the category that the consumer is inspecting, or perhaps by the consumer’s current thoughts/objectives/goals (see Huang & Lu, 2015). Indeed, it is even possible that there may be a hierarchy of associations with some being dominant over others, again possibly depending on context.

Beyond hue, it is important to note how other visual appearance properties, such as lightness/saturation (Mai et al., 2016) and glossiness (De Kerpel et al., 2020; see Spence, 2021b, for a recent review) can also convey different messages/meanings when present on product packaging. For example, light and pale colours tend to be associated with healthfulness. That being said, as Mai et al. (2016) have noted, lightness may have different meanings for different people, and the association between lightness and perceived healthiness can be moderated by other factors including the goals of the consumer. Glossiness, on the other hand, tends to be associated with greasy and/or unhealthy foods by the majority of consumers (see Spence, 2021b, for a review).

It is at around this point that one might be tempted to ask, do visual design cues, such as colour, do anything more than merely set/prime a consumer’s expectations? And here, while online research that merely assesses expectations is just so much easier to conduct (e.g. Woods et al., 2015), nevertheless there are a few studies showing how changes to the visual appearance of the receptacle in which a product is packaged can significantly influence not just people’s expectations, but also their experience (cf. Carvalho & Spence, 2019). At the same time, however, it is important to note that the power of any visual cue, such as colour, as discussed in this section, to modulate taste is dependent not only on the strength or robustness of the association between the colour and the related taste, but also the degree of discrepancy between the consumer’s expectation and their actual experience (e.g. see Schifferstein, 2001; Spence & Piqueras-Fiszman, 2012, for reviews).

Shape, packaging, and crossmodal correspondences

Given what we have seen so far, it should be clear that the shape of product packaging may convey (or prime) multiple distinct meanings to the consumer. Specific packaging shapes may be associated with quality, brand, gender, healthfulness, and strength (see Hine, 1995; Stern, 1981). And, just as for the case of colour, the various different theoretical accounts all have something to say regarding the meaning(s) of shape cues in product packaging (see Table 1). One recent area of interest amongst researchers has been on the crossmodal correspondences between shapes and taste/flavour (Velasco et al., 2016a, 2016b). The latest research has highlighted the fact that basic shape properties are associated with taste in a manner that can, at times, seem almost synaesthetic (Cytowic & Wood, 1982) though, importantly, is not (Deroy & Spence, 2013). Roundness, for example, tends to be associated with sweetness, whereas angularity tends to be associated with bitterness, sourness, and saltiness (Spence & Deroy, 2012, 2013). Sourness is also associated with asymmetrical, rather than with symmetrical, visual designs (see Salgado-Montejo et al., 2015a; Turoman et al., 2018). Given such findings, shape-taste correspondences can be incorporated into a range of design elements including everything from typeface (de Sousa et al., 2020; Mead et al., 2020; Velasco & Spence, 2019b; Wang et al., 2020) to lines and shapes on/of labels (Li et al., 2022; Matthews et al., 2019), transparent windows (Simmonds et al., 2019), and even the distinctive image moulds of specific packaging forms or silhouettes (Meyers, 1981; Overbeeke & Peters, 1991; Spence & Piqueras-Fiszman, 2012; Wang & Sun, 2006). While certain shapes are associated with specific flavours, atypical food packaging might attract attention and increase product salience (cf. van Ooijen et al., 2016). However, as the latter researchers point out, atypical packaging can also have a detrimental effect on the consumer’s product evaluation. Specifically, it can enhance the processing of product information which, in turn, decreases the persuasiveness of weak (i.e. unconvincing) messaging.

It is currently unclear what the basis of shape/taste associations might be (Dichter, 1971; Gal et al., 2007; Obrist et al., 2014; Spence & Deroy, 2012, 2013). According to one suggestion, it may simply be that pleasant shapes are linked with pleasant tastes (e.g. round with sweet) while potentially threatening stimuli (e.g. angular shapes and bitterness) may be grouped together. One can think of this as a kind of emotional mediation, or affective correspondence, account (Salgado-Montejo et al., 2015a). However, according to Obrist et al. (2014), roundness may be associated with sweetness because of the gradual change in taste sensation that is experienced with this kind of taste stimulus. Obrist et al. demonstrated that people typically experience sweetness as building slowly, having a rounded or smoothed peak, and then decaying slowly on the palate. By contrast, sour tastes are experienced as having a much sharper temporal onset and offset. That said, the fact that many crossmodal correspondences have been incorporated conventionally in product packaging for decades, means it is hard to discount the possibility that consumers have simply internalized (perhaps unconsciously) the regularities of the marketplace.

Cross-cultural research from Bremner et al. (2013) is of relevance here. These researchers investigated the Himba tribe in Namibia. These hunter-gatherers have no written language nor access to supermarkets. Intriguingly, this group does not show the same taste-shape correspondences that have been documented elsewhere. Specifically, they exhibited no association between angularity and carbonation in sparkling (vs. still) water (cf. Spence, 2019a). What is more, they associated milk chocolate (i.e. sweet) with angular shapes while matching dark chocolate (i.e. bitter) with round shapes—the opposite of what has been demonstrated repeatedly elsewhere. This suggests that the internalization of the visual communication conventions of the marketplace may well play an important role in explaining certain crossmodal correspondences relevant to product packaging (and/or product forms). Notice here how, should such idiosyncratic results be replicated, they would argue against Obrist et al.’s (2014) putative account of taste-shape correspondences. The various explanations (see Table 1) for the communicative function of shape cues should not, of course, be treated as mutually exclusive, and indeed several explanations have been shown to contribute to explaining a number of the crossmodal correspondences that have been documented in the literature to date (Spence, 2020a).

Shape may also be associated with health, strength, or possibly even with taste properties (Parise & Spence, 2012). There is also a literature on branded ‘image moulds’: That is, distinctive packaging forms or silhouettes (Arboleda & Arce-Lopera, 2015) that may become associated with a specific brand (e.g. consider only the contour of a Coca-Cola bottle; Prince, 1994) and/or with a specific class of product (Söderlund et al., 2017), as happened some years ago with the sloped-shouldered Wishbone salad dressing bottle (see Hine, 1995; Meyers, 1981). The suggestion is that the most successful packaging forms have become image moulds in lieu of the fact that the shape features (e.g. rounded or angular) are consistent with the key brand attributes (Anon., 1994; Gislason et al., 2020; Parise & Spence, 2012). On occasion, semantically meaningful shapes have been incorporated in packaging design (e.g. as in the successful case of the green tea sold in Japan in a green plastic bottle that itself resembles bamboo; see Visser, 2009, pp. 8–9).

Importantly, and just as was the case for colour (discussed earlier), given that packaging shapes are associated with a variety of different attributes, consumers may need to be primed to think about taste (gustation) before they discriminate between shapes as a function of taste. That is, consumer goals (or context) may be critical in terms of determining the communicative function of packaging shape. That said, and again, there may also be a hierarchy of values. Addressing these issues constitutes an important task for future applied packaging research. And, once again, future research might benefit from considering how Mukherjee et al.’s (2022) theory of semantic discriminability. In particular, it would be interesting to know more about the role of context, or comparison stimuli, in determining whether the concepts that are primed in the consumer’s mind by specific shape cues might not reflect inference rather than necessarily direction association.

When orientation biases meaning

The orientation of abstract visual design features (such as shapes) on product packaging also matters when it comes to communicating with the consumer. For instance, people have been shown to respond very differently to triangles as a function of whether they happen to point upwards or downwards (Zhao et al., 2017, 2020). Triangles, or other angular shapes, that pointing downwards/towards the viewer can trigger a short-lasting neural fear response in the human amygdala (Larson et al., 2007; Watson et al., 2011). One explanation that has been put forward for this finding is that downward-pointing, relative to upward-pointing, triangles generate a change in visual processing that is driven by negative affective properties (Watson et al., 2011).

Meanwhile, lines ascending to the right have very different connotations than when the same line ascends to the left instead (see Spence et al., 2019, for a review). The former appear to be associated with positive dynamism, whereas the latter tend to have a much less positive connotation (see Velasco et al., 2015c). By way of example, Mead et al. (2020) reported that right-slanted fonts were effective in evoking thoughts of an advertising campaign that was moving forward (and thus that time was running out) which, in turn, influenced people’s purchasing intentions. Intriguingly, it has even been suggested that the response to oriented lines can appear almost innate (see Karim et al., 2016).

Notice here also how, depending on its orientation, the same curved line may look like a smile or a frown (Salgado-Montejo et al., 2015b). Even the direction in which individual faces are looking (i.e. to the left or right) has been shown to subtly prime different expectations/associations in the mind of the consumer (Park et al., 2021). Specifically, leftward-facing people are deemed to be ever-so-slightly more attractive which, in turn, has been shown to promote more positive attitudes towards products.

As another example, the customers in one intriguing study were invited to evaluate the ‘house blend’ of coffee (Van Rompay et al., 2019, based on work by Rorink, 2018). These authors established that horizontal vs. vertical stripes on a poster in a Dutch coffee shop influenced customers’ ratings of the coffee. Van Rompay et al. used the concept of ‘embodied cognition’ to help explain their findings. Specifically, their suggestion was that luxury and power are associated with ‘top-shelf’ and ‘looking down’ on others, respectively. These researchers reported that vertical, relative to horizontal, stripes positively influenced taste experience, quality perception, and purchase intention of coffee. The argument is that a vertically oriented advertising display may invoke perceptions of power (i.e. Machiels & Orth, 2017; Schubert, 2005; Sundar & Noseworthy, 2014; van Rompay et al., 2012). The suggestion was that this, in turn, caused the consumers to rate the coffee as having a more powerful/intense taste, relative to those in a horizontally oriented advertisement condition.

Position implicitly conveys meaning

Researchers have explored other indicators of verticality, such as the positioning of elements on product packaging. For instance, Fenko et al. (2018) assessed the impact of incorporating an image of a lion (as a metaphor for strength) on a package of coffee beans. The lion could either appear at the top or bottom of the packaging. The lion’s location was shown to influence both multisensory flavour perception and purchase intentions. When the image was situated at the bottom of the packaging, the coffee was perceived to be stronger. Fenko and her colleagues argued that this is consistent with the theory of grounded cognition, whereby a ‘strong is heavy’ metaphor is activated, with heavy objects usually located on the ground. Similarly, Togawa et al. (2019) found that an image of a food item placed lower on the product packaging enhanced both people’s expectations and perceptions of the heaviness of the product’s flavour. Interestingly, the association between position and heaviness influenced consumers’ decisions regarding healthy eating, such that they consumed less of the ‘heavy’ food and tended to choose a healthier snack option instead.

In research exploring the association between healthiness and vertical position, Wang and Basso (2021) recently demonstrated that people associate healthy food (i.e. fruit salad) with high verticality, whereas unhealthy food (i.e. ice cream) was associated with low verticality instead. These researchers suggested that conceptual metaphor theory could be used to explain their findings in that health is commonly associated with ‘up’ (being upright; sayings such as ‘She is in peak physical condition’), while illness is associated with ‘down’ (being forced to lie down in bed; ‘She felt under the weather’ or being ‘down in the dumps’). Meanwhile, in an earlier study, Deng and Kahn (2009) reported that the consumer’s goals (e.g. to be healthy) influenced their preferences for the location of objects on product packaging. Specifically, those consumers with a health goal exhibited a weakened preference for packages where the image was situated at the bottom (i.e. heavy location). While the design features whose position has been varied were semantically meaningful stimuli in the above-mentioned cases, it might be expected that similar associations would be documented were it to be the position of an abstract visual design element that was varying instead.

Elsewhere, Simmonds et al. (2018b) demonstrated that the left/right position of transparent windows embedded in product packaging significantly influenced ratings of a range of product qualities (e.g. overall liking, quality, willingness to purchase) for fake brands of lemon mousse, cereal, and chocolate. Finally here, mention should be made of Salgado-Montejo et al. (2015b) study highlighting how the position of a concave/convex line on the front of product packaging (top, middle, or bottom) biased the likelihood with which that design feature was interpreted as a smile. Specifically, the line was more likely to be interpreted as a smile when it appeared at the bottom, rather than the top, of product packaging, thus suggesting a degree of anthropomorphism. Note here that anthropomorphism in product/packaging design tends to increase consumer preference (Batra et al., 2016). Similar benefits have now been noted across a wide range of product categories (e.g. Rapaille, 2007; Wang & Basso, 2019).

Interim summary

An emerging body of scientific research has started to document the various meanings that are associated (by consumers) with specific visual cues/design features in product packaging in the food and beverage category. Colours (and saturation, lightness, and finish/glossiness) on product packaging have all been associated with various taste/flavour properties, product quality, and the healthiness of the product contained within the packaging. Stripes, be they vertical or horizontal, represent an interesting class of design feature in not having a clear connotative meaning (Albertazzi et al., 2021; Walker & Walker, 2012) established in the literature to date. In contrast to other design features mentioned so far, stripes represent an abstract visual design feature that has (to date at least) seemingly received little research attention from those interested in product packaging (see, for example, the absence of coverage in Velasco & Spence, 2019a), despite various companies choosing to introduce stripes in their product packaging.

Combining abstract visual design features

Having reviewed the evidence concerning the meaning of individual abstract visual design cues, such as colour, shape, and orientation in product packaging, it seems worthwhile turning to the question of how various combinations of abstract visual design cues may be interpreted by the consumer. This can either be combinations of colours, as in colour pairs or triplets, or combinations of different visual features, such as the combination of colour and shape. However, given the combinatorial explosion that one is soon faced with when combining different visual design features, our focus in this section will be on the associations/meaning that may be associated with, or primed by stripes, given their neglect in the literature on crossmodal correspondences to date, together with their frequent appearance in nature and product packaging.

The use of stripes introduces combinations of visual features such as colour pairs which, in turn, might be expected to generate interesting effects such as colour contrast. The meaning of colour pairs has been well-studied but depends, to a certain degree, on the specific relation between the component parts. For example, side-by-side vs. foreground/background arrangements will need to be considered by package designers, and even which element is in the fore-/back-ground (Woods & Spence, 2016; Woods et al., 2016; cf. Deng et al., 2010; Schifferstein & Howell, 2015). Pink on a white background, for example, is more strongly associated with sweetness than (a) when either colour is presented in isolation, (b) when white is presented against a pink background, or (c) when these colours are presented side-by-side instead.

There is a longstanding, separate literature on colour-shape correspondences (e.g. Dreklser & Spence, 2019). The research has demonstrated that combinations of colour and form sometimes take on specific symbolic (i.e. the image/association that comes to mind with respect to a product; Kujala & Nurkka, 2012) and/or affective (i.e. the emotion elicited by a stimulus) meaning (Ares & Deliza, 2010; Kaeppler, 2018; Oyama, 2003; Spence, 2021c). One might question whether cues are combined based on similar connotative meanings, as assessed by approaches such as the semantic differential technique (Osgood et al., 1957; Snider & Osgood, 1969; cf. Henson et al., 2006; Kawachi et al., 2011; Morich, 1981; Oyama et al., 1998; Schaefer & Rotte, 2010; Suzuki et al., 2005). Consider, for example, how red and highly angular shapes often co-occur (e.g. on the front of beer cans; Spence, 2012). This constellation of abstract visual design features may go particularly well together because, when presented individually, both stimuli are associated with activity and dominance (rather than with passivity and submissiveness) according to semantic differential analysis (Adams & Osgood, 1973).

Ensuring the congruencyFootnote 6 of different visual design elements has been suggested to be an important part of successful design (Fürst et al., 2021; Heatherly et al., 2019; Matthews et al., 2019; Salgado-Montejo et al., 2014), processing fluency,Footnote 7 and effective visual search (Velasco et al., 2015a). From a marketing perspective, the wrong combination of design features can exert a drastic negative impact on brand perception and, importantly, sales. Tom et al. (1987) provide an example where a Swiss coffee brand redesigned their packaging. Although the new packaging won awards for design, sales plummeted. The problem appeared to be that diagonal stripes of mauve were simply not deemed congruent with the conventions of the category (i.e. coffee packaging) by the consumer. Favre and November (1979) provided several other historic examples of unsuccessful packaging colour rebrands. Hence, having established the connotative meaning of specific visual design features as a function of their position/orientation, manufacturers have a choice to either follow the conventions of the category or go for something different. However, only some brands seem able to carry-off incongruent signalling in the marketplace (cf. Sundar & Noseworthy, 2016), especially given the disruption to processing fluency that such incongruency is likely to elicit (Lunardo & Livat, 2016; cf. Herrmann et al., 2013; Labroo et al., 2008). Wheatley (1973) gives the example of the hugely successful Alpen muesli that came out with matte black packaging for their muesli in the 1970s in a mostly white and sunny yellow coloured product category (i.e. breakfast cereal). More recently, several fabric conditioner brands have similarly attempted to disrupt the colour conventions of the laundry category by again coming out with black packaging in a mostly white and blue packaging colour category. It is interesting to consider here how the desire to stand out on the shelf, and so capture the customer’s visual attention more effectively (see Reutskaja et al., 2011; Spence & Piqueras-Fiszman, 2012), often leads to the colour (and other visual design) conventions of the category being overturned. This strategy has been used very effectively in recent years in the drinks category, by those such as Gatorade, and more recently, Innocent (the latter with their Bolt from the Blue product launch; see Spence, 2021d).

Certain combinations of shapes and colours may take on symbolic or semantic associations. Think, for example, of how a red circle or plus sign on a field of white may prime notions of the Japanese flag and the Red Cross, respectively (Chen et al., 2021). One might also consider the semantic meaning of the iconic Lucky Strike cigarette packaging showing a red circle against a white background (designed by Raymond Loewy). Furthermore, people typically associate yellow with a crescent shape, presumably because they are reminded of the moon (Dreksler & Spence, 2019; Woods et al., 2013). It is worth noting that combinations of colours in stripes can be associated with a particular (semantic) meaning which is, at times, dependent on the orientation of the stripes (see below).

Recent commercial examples: on the use and orientation of coloured stripes on product packaging

Given Kentucky Fried Chicken’s (KFC’s) recent decision to update the design of their food product packaging (Anon., 2021) and stores (Valinsky, 2020) to emphasize their signature vertical red-and-white stripes, we have chosen to use them as a recent commercial example regarding the use of stripes in product branding and how these design elements contribute to perception. Similarly, Devondale—a company offering a range of dairy products—updated their packaging back in 2012 such that it included horizontal light blue-and-white stripes (Hicks, 2012). The branding on this iconic Australian range of dairy products is reminiscent of the famous ‘Cornishware’ style of English kitchen pottery. Of relevance, given Devondale’s use of blue-and-white stripes, and the fact that the company has ties to dairy farming, this design may also be intended to evoke thoughts of farms, cottages, and cows (see also Rodionova, 2016, for supermarkets attempting to create associations by using fake farm names).

Note here how the semantic/affective associations with horizontal light blue-and-white stripes cannot simply be predicted based on the consumer’s response to the individual abstract visual design cues (Spence et al., 2015a, b). One might consider whether the blue caps on traditional milk bottles could also provide a basis for the use of this combination of colours (i.e. blue cap plus white milk). Abstract patches of blue and white, when presented together, are associated with a salty taste (Woods & Spence, 2016; Woods et al., 2016). It is, though, worth noting that the participants in Woods and colleagues’ online research were primed to think in terms of the associations between colour pairs and basic tastes, given that they were forced to choose between the basic tastes when responding (cf. Mukherjee et al., 2022). Thus, even though the combination of blue and white may be more strongly associated with salty than with other tastes, that does not preclude the possibility that the consumer might be primed to think of milk/dairy more than they are to think of salt on seeing this combination of colours.

Similarly, a particular shade of purple, red, or turquoise might well be expected to prime associations with the branded colours of Cadbury’s chocolate, Coca-Cola, and Tiffany jewellery, respectively, more than with specific taste qualities (see Baxter et al., 2018). It would be helpful if future research, in which the associations primed in consumers by viewing specific combinations of colours, were not constrained by a forced-choice design (e.g. as in the open responding required in the Word Association task, for example; Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2013). At the same time, given the multitude of responses that might ensue, it would perhaps help to give the consumer a particular context (e.g. ‘What comes to mind if you saw this particular combination of colours in the refrigerated section of a supermarket?’). A third example of the use of coloured stripes in product packing relates to the LGBTQI + movement and the incorporation of rainbow stripes into product design (e.g. Ralph Lauren t-shirts, ADIDAS shoes; see Yates, 2021, for a number of other examples). By way of example, the incorporation of the LGBTQI + flag, which combines five colours, into product design may have implications for the connotative meaning and brand perception beyond the associations primed by colours.

In relation to KFC and Devondale, products in warm-coloured packaging (e.g. red) are deemed to be less healthy than are those presented in packages using cooler colours (e.g. blue; see Singh, 2006; Van Rompay et al., 2016). Woods et al. (2016) demonstrated that colour pairs communicate basic tastes and found that, for example, the pairing of white and red was associated with saltiness. This is interesting considering KFC’s recent rebrand where the red-and-white stripes were made more vibrant. Woods et al. also reported that the combination of white and blue better portrayed saltiness than when using either colour alone; previous research had shown that each colour was associated with saltiness (Favre & November, 1979; Spence et al., 2015b; cf. Velasco et al., 2016a, 2016b). It might seem odd then that Devondale should choose to use this pairing on dairy products as, although butter is often salted (but can also be unsalted), a company might want to avoid generating an expectation of salty milk. Perhaps the hope is to generate mental imagery associated with Cornishware in those who happen to be familiar with this famous traditional style of pottery from the UK, and that this will override the blue-white/saltiness correspondence (at least in those who are familiar with Cornishware).

A separate literature has explored the perceptual differences generated by lines as a function of whether they are shown horizontally vs. vertically (Avery & Day, 1969). People tend to perceive horizontal lines as being shorter than lines of equivalent length presented vertically. Such visual illusions have implications for the form (or orientation) of product packaging in that consumers perceive short, wide packages to hold less volume than tall, slender packages (see Chen & Shi, 2017; see also Raghubir & Greenleaf, 2006); Raghubir & Krishna, 1999; cf. Cheskin, 1951, pp. 193–194). Think, here, only of Piaget’s conservation task (Piaget, 1952). To add another layer of complexity to this issue, the ‘Helmholtz Square’ illusion shows that a square comprised of vertical stripes appears to be shorter and wider than an identical square comprising horizontal stripes (Coren & Girgus, 1978; Seriously Science, 2014; Thompson & Mikellidou, 2009). In all the above cases, while the shape itself does not change, simply altering the orientation leads to a predictable change in visual perception. Interestingly, this may be of benefit to Devondale in their marketing of butter which is presented in rectangular containers. Although speculative, the Devondale container with its horizontal stripes might make the container look taller, thus creating an illusion such that people unconsciously believe they are getting more for their money.

Thus, the orientation of visual design elements on product packaging, and even the position of packaging on shelving (see Sunaga et al., 2016, on the lightness-elevation correspondence that can be used to guide shelf positioning), influences perception by priming those attributes that happen to be linked to specific visual design features. Hence, KFC which achieved success via the introduction of vertical red-and-white stripes (see Anon., 2021), and Devondale who use horizontal blue-and-white stripes on their brand packaging, may succeed independently of one another due to the influence of several, independent factors helping to determine/constitute the meaning of coloured stripes.

Assessing the effectiveness of stripes in product packaging

Evolutionary theory provides a possible, if highly speculative, explanatory framework for the success/appeal of stripes in the marketing of products in the food and beverage category (see above for the evolutionary account of several other visual design features). Although it is beyond the scope of this narrative review to comprehensively list all of the hypotheses relating to communicating signals, we outline a few particularly relevant ones below. The first thing to point out is that repetitious patterns (e.g. stripes) are common in nature—think of the zebra, zebra fish, or tiger snake, as examples (Coborn, 1991; Lieske & Myers, 1994). Repetitive patterns may have evolved in nature to stimulate ‘the receiver regardless of the position of the signal’ (Kenward et al., 2004, p. 412) on the retina. At the same time, however, the incorporation of stripes may serve somewhat different functions in different species. The zebra’s distinctive stripes, for example, help to deter flies from biting them (see How et al., 2020), while tigers might have stripes to hide/for camouflage, and bees perhaps to warn off other creatures (though see Stelzer et al., 2010, for evidence questioning the latter suggestion). In other words, the effect of stripes might rely on the qualities of the stripes, the combination of colours used, and the ecological niche inhabited by the animal.

In much the same way, it has been suggested that the presence of stripes might be used for camouflage or capture attention, colour is important both as an aid to foraging (Foroni et al., 2016)—though researchers argue about whether it developed to facilitate frugivory or folivory (e.g. Sumner & Mollon, 2003)—as well as a potential signal for mating/conspecific communication (e.g. Changizi et al., 2006; see also Humphrey, 1976, on the complex evolutionary meanings associated with colour). Hence, evolutionary accounts are currently both limited in the range of visual design cues that they can potentially provide an explanation for, and are often open to other interpretations (both evolutionary and otherwise), meaning that they are of only limited explanatory validity in decoding the visual aspects of packaging design.

Repetitive patterns such as stripes may be used in marketing because images created on the retina can vary in orientation as well as in position. Think, for example, of a shopper in a supermarket moving past a product from right-to-left, and then from left-to-right. This creates image sequences that are mirror reflections of each other. Stripes will be invariant when reflected (i.e. symmetrical), so the use of stripes may contribute to enhanced processing fluency (cf. Bigoin-Gagnan & Lacoste-Badie, 2018). Manufacturers of sour products may want to avoid the use of stripes though, given that sourness tends to be associated with visual designs that are asymmetrical (Salgado-Montejo et al., 2015a; Turoman et al., 2018). Remember here how a lack of congruency between visual design elements and expected taste attributes can negatively impact product attitudes (see Ares & Deliza, 2010). At the same time, however, it is worth noting that symmetry is processed fluently, and hence tends to be preferred visually (Pecchinenda et al., 2014).

One of the problems with products on supermarket shelves is the need to stand out when parts of the packaging may be obscured. Importantly, repetitive patterns such as stripes may be useful because they look similar even when parts of the product (or animal in nature) are obscured and, as such, will still be recognizable to an onlooker (Kenward et al., 2004). An important physiological process explaining the usefulness of stripes might be lateral inhibition. Lateral inhibition is defined as ‘the capacity of excited neurons to reduce the activity of their neighbours’ (Cohen, 2011, p. 1437). Lateral inhibition helps to enhance edges, and ‘makes it easier to distinguish objects from backgrounds under varying light conditions’ (Kenward et al., 2004, p. 415). As such, stripes have a greater apparent maximum intensity than do solid blocks of colour, and thus they tend to ‘pop out’. As brands have very limited time to attract the attention of potential buyers (Reutskaja et al., 2011; Sugermeyer, 2021), stripes might work well amongst the myriad products and advertising clutter on shelves. At a psychological level, one might also choose to invoke Gestalt theory to help describe the factors affecting the grouping of elements, such as lines, in product packaging (Ellis, 1938; Wagemans, 2015). Grouping principles such as grouping-by-similarity, grouping-by-proximity, and good continuation may sometimes also help to predict/explain why visual design features, such as stripes, are grouped in certain ways.

Finally, it is worth noting that stripes may also have other semantic associations, that have been built up through experience, and which may help to explain their meaning to consumers (i.e. independent of any specific evolutionary account). Consumers might, for instance, be primed by the sight of black and white stripes to think of prison uniforms, or perhaps a fashion icon such as Coco Chanel, or a sports team (e.g. Juventus). There are, in other words, likely always going to be a range of explanations behind the ‘meaning(s)’, or associations, that happen to be primed by any given visual design feature. It is important to note that the various explanations should not be treated as mutually exclusive, and indeed several explanations have recently been shown to contribute to explaining many of the crossmodal correspondences that have been documented in the literature (Spence, 2020a).

Interim summary

The research reported in this section highlight how the meaning attributed by consumers to the combination of different abstract visual cues, such as colour pairs or colour and shape, typically cannot simply be predicted simply on the basis of the consumers’ response to the individual visual cues when assessed in isolation. Sometimes, for example, specific combinations of visual design cues may deliver a configuration that takes on a meaning of its own, as with the thick horizontal blue and white stripes that may be associated with Cornishware pottery, while the individual colours are likely to be associated with a salty taste (see Spence et al., 2015a, b). By contrast, the red and white vertical stripes of KFC packaging might be expected to cue saltiness and power, possibly enhancing the taste of the product (cf. Fenko et al., 2016). One of the important areas for future research on the visual design of product packaging is therefore to understand more about the meaning to consumers of various combinations of abstract visual design cues (e.g. such as the combination involved in coloured stripes).

Conclusions and future directions

The majority of the research on the visual design of product packaging has addressed individual visual design features. However, while this is undoubtedly a fruitful first step, it is crucial to note that any realistic example of food or beverage packaging will inevitably incorporate several visual design elements (see Favre & November, 1979; Hine, 1995; Visser, 2009). Hence, the question immediately becomes one of whether it is possible to predict the consumer’s response to the combination based simply on how they respond to individual abstract visual cues, such as colour or shape/form (Labrecque et al., 2013). The limited evidence that has been published to date certainly suggests that while abstract visual design elements that are congruent in terms of their connotative meaning, and/or that are linked by their crossmodal correspondence, are sometimes combined, there are other situations in which a specific configuration of visual design cues takes on a semantic meaning that goes beyond the meaning of the individual cues (Dreksler & Spence, 2019; Matthews et al., 2019; Spence, 2020a; Zhao et al., 2020; and see Velasco et al., 2014a, for a review). It should, of course, further be remembered that visual design cues are but one element of multisensory packaging design.

Furthermore, although several studies have shown that individual visual design features (e.g. colours) have similar meanings across cultures (Adams & Osgood, 1973; Wheatley, 1973), some of the meanings (or codes) of packaging would appear to be market specific (Velasco et al., 2014b). This is obviously an important area for future research as far as international brands are concerned. However, returning to a point we made a moment ago, there is currently very limited evidence assessing whether combinations of features influence consumers from different cultures in similar ways (see Van Doorn et al., 2017, for one example relating to the influence of the height and width of coffee cups). One intriguing recent approach to establishing the affective or connotative colour associations has been based on machine learning (e.g. Jahanian et al., 2017; Jonauskaite et al., 2019b; see also Schloss et al., 2019).

At times, of course, design features are incorporated to make products stand out, and thus facilitate visual search for product packaging (e.g. Jansson, Marlow, & Bristow, 2004; Shen et al., 2015; Velasco et al., 2015a; Zhao et al., 2017). Importantly, this can help to facilitate information processing (van Ooijen et al., 2016) but, depending on how design features are integrated, also has the potential to negatively impact product evaluations (Spence & Piqueras-Fiszman, 2012; Sundar & Noseworthy, 2016). There is also a growing awareness that certain visual designs that have been shown to work well in the setting of physical bricks and mortar store may need to be modified/simplified to maximize their appeal for the online shopping setting (Reinoso-Carvalho et al., 2021).

As our understanding of the meaning, or connotation, of visual design elements of product packaging in the food and beverage category continues to grow, based on the theory of crossmodal correspondences, there is an opportunity to predictively develop packaging that has been optimized to combine visual features such as colour, shape, orientation, and position in order to convey the appropriate meaning (Jacquot et al., 2016; Velasco et al., 2014a; see Spence, 2020a, for a review) and/or capture the consumer’s attention. On occasion, visual design elements may be combined in an attempt to capture the consumer’s attention (see Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2012) but, given the likely loss of processing fluency (Labroo et al., 2008; cf. Dohle & Siegrist, 2014), this technique should be used cautiously. Of course, any change in product/packaging design may lead to success simply because it is novel and/or captures the shopper’s visual attention (as in the case of the Bolt from the Blue from Innocent Drinks; Spence, 2021d). However, altering iconic visual designs can all-too-easily lead to a backlash from consumers that can adversely affect sales. PepsiCo discovered this some years ago when they changed their iconic ‘straw in a juicy orange’ design on their Tropicana packaging (Airey, 2010; Marion, 2015; see also Favre & November, 1979).Footnote 8

As highlighted by this review, the scientific approach to visual design of food and beverage product packaging is rapidly contributing knowledge in this field, and helping product designers/marketers to significantly increase sales (Sugermeyer, 2021). The emerging understanding of the connotative meaning/crossmodal correspondences that are associated with specific abstract visual design cues, such as colour, shape, orientation, and position means that it is increasingly possible to predictively prime certain attributes. At the same time, however, most product packaging incorporates a variety of design elements, and their meaning, in combination, is not always easy to predict from elements studied in isolation. There is, therefore, a danger of combinatorial explosion should one try to map out the meaning of a wide array of combinations of design features. At the same time, as should have become apparent from the above discussion, researchers and practitioners still remain a long way from developing a commonly agreed account of visual design. Perhaps, though, this should not come as any surprise, given the variety of signs and contexts evoked by the visual design of food and beverage, or for that matter, any other category, of packaging.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. Synaesthesia refers to the phenomenon whereby stimulating one sense (or sensory dimension) leads to automatic, involuntary, and idiosyncratic perceptual experiences in a second sense, or sensory dimension (cf. Oyama et al., 1998).

  2. Note that these terms are used scientifically, as opposed to colloquially, usage. As such taste is used to refer to one of the gustatorily determined basic tastes (e.g. sweet, sour, bitter, salty, sour, and umami), whereas flavour refers to the combined experience of taste and smell as in the experience of citrus, fruity, floral, or herbal notes (see Spence et al., 2015a, b).

  3. Abstract visual design features/properties in packaging design include any simple feature (such as a colour, shape, and visual texture) that does not have an obvious semantic meaning. Note that while signature hues associated with brands might well be said to represent a simple design feature that has become imbued with semantic meaning (i.e. whatever the consumer associates with the brand, e.g. Baxter et al., 2018), such individual hues, along with other specific colours, will be treated as abstract visual design features here.

  4. According to Lafontaine et al. (2020, p. 244): ‘Associative learning is defined as learning about the relationship between two separate stimuli, where the stimuli might range from concrete objects and events to abstract concepts, such as time, location, context, or categories’.

  5. Here, already, one might start to wonder whether inferred colour-concept relations (see Tham et al., 2020) are as effective in terms of consumer perception/behaviour as the inferred mappings that are presumably often picked-up by the research (see Spence & Levitan, 2022).

  6. While congruency is a challenging notion to define, it is generally taken to refer to combinations of features or attributes that are perceived as ‘going well together’, possibly because the elements commonly co-occur, or because they share perceptual affinity/similarity (Amsellem & Ohla, 2016; though see Schifferstein & Verlegh, 1996).

  7. The widely used notion of ‘processing fluency’ refers to the ease with which a given stimulus configuration can be processed. Processing fluency tends to be higher for those stimuli that are familiar, easy to process, and where the component stimuli are congruent. Processing fluency is positively valenced (Lunardo & Livat, 2016; cf. Labroo et al., 2008).

  8. One might consider this slightly ironic given that Tropicana source their oranges from Florida where, traditionally, most oranges were the green-skinned variety (see Hisano, 2019).

References

  • Adams, F. M., & Osgood, C. E. (1973). A cross-cultural study of the affective meanings of color. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 4, 135–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Airey, D. (2010). Logo design love: A guide to creating iconic brand identities. Berkeley, CA: New Riders.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albertazzi, L., Canal, L., Micciolo, R., & Hachen, I. (2021). The perceptual organisation of visual elements: Lines. Brain Sciences, 11, 1585. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121585

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Amsellem, S., & Ohla, K. (2016). Perceived odor-taste congruence influences intensity and pleasantness differently. Chemical Senses, 41(8), 677–684. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjw078

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amsteus, M., Al-Shaaban, S., Wallin, E., & Sjöqvist, S. (2015). Colors in marketing: A study of color associations and context (in) dependence. International Journal of Business and Social Science, 6(3), 32–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anon. (1994). New bottle for Listerine. Marketing News, 28, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anon. (2021). KFC reveals refreshed packaging design. Packaging Europe, May 26th. https://packagingeurope.com/kfc-reveals-refreshed-packaging-design/

  • Arboleda, A. M., & Arce-Lopera, C. (2015). Quantitative analysis of product categorization in soft drinks using bottle silhouettes. Food Quality & Preference, 45, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ares, G., & Deliza, R. (2010). Studying the influence of package shape and colour on consumer expectations of milk desserts using word association and conjoint analysis. Food Quality and Preference, 21, 930–937.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aslam, M. M. (2006). Are you selling the right colour? A cross-cultural review of colour as a marketing cue. Journal of Marketing Communications, 12, 15–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Associated Press. (2013). Evian revamps ‘old and dated’ bottle after brand falls behind in the designer water market (but will anyone spot the difference?). Daily Mail Online, May 22nd. http://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-2329202/Evian-revamps-old-dated-bottle-brand-falls-designer-water-market.html

  • Avery, G. C., & Day, R. H. (1969). Basis of the horizontal-vertical illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 376–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Azzi, A., Battini, D., Persona, A., & Sgarbossa, F. (2012). Packaging design: General framework and research agenda. Packaging Technology and Science, 25, 435–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baptista, I., Spence, C., Shimizu, R., Ferreira, E., & Behrens, J. (submitted). Colour is to flavour what shape is to texture: A choice-based conjoint study of visual cues on chocolate packaging. Food Research International.

  • Baptista, I., Valentin, D., Saldaña Villa, E., & Herman Behrens, J. (2021). Effects of packaging color on expected flavor, texture and liking of chocolate in Brazil and France. International Journal of Gastronomy and Food Science, 24(8), 100340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgfs.2021.100340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbosa Escobar, F., Wang, Q. J., Byrne, D. V., Corredor, A., & Velasco, C. (2022). The taste of visual textures. Food Quality & Preference. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2022.104602.

  • Barthel, D. (1989). Modernism and marketing: The chocolate box revisited. Theory, Culture and Society, 6, 429–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barthes, R. (1977). Elements of semiology. Hill & Wang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batra, R., Seifert, C., & Brei, D. (Eds.). (2016). The psychology of design: Creating consumer appeal. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baxter, S. M., Ilicic, J., & Kulczynski, A. (2018). Roses are red, violets are blue, sophisticated brands have a Tiffany hue: The effect of iconic brand color priming on brand personality judgments. Journal of Brand Management, 25, 384–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bigoin-Gagnan, A., & Lacoste-Badie, S. (2018). Symmetry influences packaging aesthetic evaluation and purchase intention. International Journal of Retail Distribution Management, 46, 1026–1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowcott, O. (2013). Chocs in the dock: Cadbury loses case. The Guardian, October 5th. http://www.theguardian.com/business/2013/oct/04/cadbury-dairy-milk-purple-trademark-blocked

  • Bremner, A., Caparos, S., Davidoff, J., de Fockert, J., Linnell, K., & Spence, C. (2013). Bouba and Kiki in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape-sound matches, but different shape-taste matches to Westerners. Cognition, 126, 165–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.007

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carvalho, F. M., & Spence, C. (2019). Cup colour influences consumers’ expectations and experience on tasting specialty coffee. Food Quality & Preference, 75, 157–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catterall, M., & Maclaran, P. (2006). Focus groups in marketing research. In R. W. Belk (Ed.), Handbook of qualitative research methods in marketing (pp. 255–267). Edward Elgar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavallo, C., & Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2017). Visual elements of packaging shaping healthiness evaluations of consumers: The case of olive oil. Journal of Sensory Studies, 32, e12246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, D. (2017). Semiotics: The basics. Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Changizi, M. A., Zhang, Q., & Shimojo, S. (2006). Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision. Biology Letters, 2, 217–221.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, N., Watanabe, K., & Wada, M. (2021). People with high autistic traits show less consensual crossmodal correspondences between visual features and tastes. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 714277. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.714277

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Y.-L., & Shi, L.-J. (2017). Effects of container elongation on consumers’ volume perception. Neuropsychiatry, 7(4), 342–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheskin, L. (1951). Colours, and what they can do. London, UK: Blandford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheskin, L. (1957). How to predict what people will buy. Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheskin, L. (1967). Secrets of marketing success: An expert’s view on the science and art of persuasive selling. Trident Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheskin, L. (1972). Marketing success: How to achieve it. Cahners Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheskin, L. (1981). Research design and analysis in the testing of symbols. In W. Stern (Ed.), Handbook of package design research (pp. 211–220). Wiley Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coborn, J. (1991). Atlas of snakes of the world. TFH Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. A. (2011). Lateral inhibition. In J. S. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca, & B. Caplan (Eds.), Encyclopaedia of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 1436–1437). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, G. (1996). Pepsi turns air blue as color wars reach for the sky. The Independent, April 2nd. Accessed January 3, 2014 from http://www.independent.co.uk/news/pepsi-turns-air-blue-as-cola-wars-reach-for-sky-1302822.html

  • Coren, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1978). Seeing is deceiving: The psychology of visual illusions. Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crilly, N., Moultrie, J., & Clarkson, P. J. (2004). Seeing things: Consumer response to the visual domain in product design. Design Studies, 25, 547–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crisinel, A.-S., & Spence, C. (2012). Assessing the appropriateness of “synaesthetic” messaging on product packaging. Food Quality and Preference, 26, 45–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutolo, M. (29 March, 2021). This is what milk label colors really mean. Accessed June 18, 2021 from https://www.rd.com/article/milk-label-colors/

  • Cytowic, R. E., & Wood, F. B. (1982). Synaesthesia II: Psychophysical relations in the synaesthesia of geometrically shaped taste and colored hearing. Brain and Cognition, 1, 36–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danesi, M. (2013). Semiotizing a product into a brand. Social Semiotics, 23, 464–476. https://doi.org/10.1080/10350330.2013.799003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danger, E. P. (1968). Using colour to sell. Gower Technical Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danger, E. P. (1987). Selecting colour for packaging. Gower Technical Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Kerpel, L., Volles, B. K., & Van Kerckhove, A. (2020). Fats are glossy, but does glossiness imply fatness? The influence of packaging glossiness on food perceptions. Foods, 9, 90. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9010090

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • de Sousa, M. M. M., Carvalho, F. M., & Pereira, R. G. F. A. (2020). Do typefaces of packaging labels influence consumers’ perception of specialty coffee? A preliminary study. Journal of Sensory Studies, 35(5), 312599. https://doi.org/10.1111/joss.12599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deng, X., Hui, S. K., & Hutchinson, W. (2010). Consumer preferences for color combinations: An empirical analysis of similarity-based color relationships. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20, 476–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deng, X., & Kahn, B. E. (2009). Is your product on the right side? The “location effect” on perceived product heaviness and package evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research, 46, 725–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Déribéré, M. (1978). Relationship between perfumes and colors. Color Research & Application, 3, 115–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deroy, O., & Spence, C. (2013). Why we are not all synesthetes (not even weakly so). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 643–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dichter, E. (1971). The strategy of selling with packaging. Package Engineering Magazine, July, 16a–16c.

  • Dichter, E. (1975). Packaging: The sixth sense? A guide to identifying consumer motivation. Cahners Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dohle, S., & Siegrist, M. (2014). Fluency of pharmaceutical drug names predicts perceived hazardousness, assumed sides effects and willingness to buy. Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 1241–1249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dreksler, N., & Spence, C. (2019). A critical analysis of colour-shape correspondences: Examining the replicability of colour-shape associations. i-Perception, 10(1), 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2012). Color-in-context theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 61–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, W. D. (1938). A source book of Gestalt psychology. Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Favre, J. P. (1968). La couleur dans la publicité [Colour in publicity]. Couleurs, 72, 22–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Favre, J.-P., & November, A. (1979). Colour and communication. ABC-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenko, A., de Vries, R., & van Rompay, T. (2018). How strong is your coffee? The influence of visual metaphors and textual claims on consumers’ flavor perception and product evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 53. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00053

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fenko, A., Lotterman, H., & Galetzka, M. (2016). What’s in a name? The effects of sound symbolism and package shape on consumer responses to food products. Food Quality & Preference, 51, 100–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.02.021

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foroni, F., Pergola, G., & Rumiati, R. I. (2016). Food color is in the eye of the beholder: The role of human trichromatic vision in food evaluation. Scientific Reports, 6, 37034. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep37034

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fürst, A., Pecornik, N., & Binder, C. (2021). All or nothing in sensory marketing: Must all or only some sensory attributes be congruent with a product’s primary function? Journal of Retailing, 97(3), 439–458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretai.2020.09.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gal, D., Wheeler, S. C., & Shiv, B. (2007, unpublished manuscript). Cross-modal influences on gustatory perception. SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1030197

  • Garber, L. L., Jr., Hyatt, E. M., & Boya, Ü. Ö. (2008). The mediating effects of the appearance of nondurable consumer goods and their packaging on consumer behavior. In H. N. J. Schifferstein & P. Hekkert (Eds.), Product experience (pp. 581–602). Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, B. B., & Levy, S. J. (1955). The product and the brand. Harvard Business Review, 33, 33–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gil-Pérez, I., Rebollar, R., Lidón, I., Martín, J., van Trijp, H. C. M., & Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2019). Hot or not? Conveying sensory information on food packaging through the spiciness-shape correspondence. Food Quality and Preference, 71, 197–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.07.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gislason, S., Bruhn, S., Christensen, A. M., Christensen, M. T., Hansen, M. G., Kha, T. T., & Giacalone, D. (2020). The influence of bottle design on perceived quality of beer: A conjoint analytic study. Beverages, 6, 64. https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages6040064

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, R. F. (2016). The architect who persuaded McDonald's to keep its famous golden arches... because they look like a 'pair of nourishing breasts'. Daily Mail Online, July 28th. http://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article3712947/ThestrangelyFreudianstoryMcDonaldsusesgoldenarcheslogo.html

  • Hagtvedt, H. (2014). Dark is durable, light is convenient: Color value influences perceived product attributes. Advances in Consumer Research, 42, 27–31 (J. Cotte & S. Wood, Eds., Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research).

  • Haverkamp, M. (2014). Synesthetic design: Handbook for a multisensory approach. Birkhäuser.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heatherly, M., Dein, M., Munafo, J. P., & Luckett, C. R. (2019). Crossmodal correspondence between color, shapes, and wine odors. Food Quality & Preference, 71, 395–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henson, B., Choo, K. W., Barnes, C. J., & Childs, T. H. C. (2006). A semantic differential study of combined visual and tactile stimuli for package design. In P. D. Bust (Ed.), Contemporary ergonomics 2006 (pp. 174–178). Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, A., Zidansek, M., Sprott, D. E., & Spangenberg, E. R. (2013). The power of simplicity: Processing fluency and the effects of olfactory cues on retail sales. Journal of Retailing, 89, 30–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, R. (21 September, 2012). Devondale rebrands. Accessed June 18, 2021 from https://mumbrella.com.au/devondale-rebrands-118114

  • Hine, T. (1995). The total package: The secret history and hidden meanings of boxes, bottles, cans, and other persuasive containers. Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hisano, A. (2019). Visualizing taste: How business changed the look of what you eat. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, H.-N., Van Doorn, G. H., Kawabe, T., Watanabe, J., & Spence, C. (2014). Colour-temperature correspondences: When reactions to thermal stimuli are influenced by colour. PLoS ONE, 9(3), e91854. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091854

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S., Hellmer, K., Johansson, M., & Gredebä, G. (2017). Itsy bitsy spider..: Infants react with increased arousal to spiders and snakes. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1710.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • How, M. J., Gonzales, D., Irwin, A., & Caro, T. (2020). Zebra stripes, tabanid biting flies and the aperture effect. Proceedings of the Royal Society B., 287, 2020152120201521. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1521

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, L., & Lu, J. (2013). When color meets health: The impact of package colors on the perception of food healthiness and purchase intention. In S. Botti & A. Labroo (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 41, pp. 625–626). Association for Consumer Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, L., & Lu, J. (2015). Eat with your eyes: Package color influences the expectation of food taste and healthiness moderated by external eating. The Marketing Management Journal, 25(2), 71–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, J., Zhao, P., & Wan, X. (2021). From brain variations to individual differences in the color–flavor incongruency effect: A combined virtual reality and resting-state fMRI study. Journal of Business Research, 123, 604–612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The colour currency of nature. In T. Porter & B. Mikelides (Eds.), Colour for architecture (pp. 95–98). Studio Vista.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. Journal of Human Evolution, 51(1), 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, L., Keown, C., Worthley, R., & Ghymn, K. I. (1991). Cross-cultural colour comparisons: Global marketers beware! International Marketing Review, 8(3), 21–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacquot, M., Velasco, C., Spence, C., & Maric, Y. (2016). On the colors of odors. Chemosensory Perception, 9, 79–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jahanian, A., Keshvari, S., Vishwanathan, S., & Allebach, J. P. (2017). Colors–messengers of concepts: Visual design mining for learning color semantics. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 24(1), 2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jansson, C., Marlow, N., & Bristow, M. (2004). The influence of colour on visual search times in cluttered environments. Journal of Marketing Communications, 10(3), 183–193. https://doi.org/10.1080/1352726042000207162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonauskaite, D., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Abu-Akel, A., Al-Rasheed, A. S., Antonietti, J.-P., Ásgeirsson, Á. G., Atitsogbe, K. A., Barma, M., Barratt, D., Bogushevskaya, V., & Meziane, M. K. B. (2019a). The sun is no fun without rain: Physical environments affect how we feel about yellow across 55 countries. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 66, 101350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonauskaite, D., Wicker, J., Mohr, C., Dael, N., Havelka, J., Papadatou-Pastou, M., Zhang, M., & Oberfeld, D. (2019b). A machine learning approach to quantify the specificity of colour–emotion associations and their cultural differences. Royal Society Open Science, 6(9), 190741.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kaeppler, K. (2018). Crossmodal associations between olfaction and vision: Color and shape visualizations of odors. Chemosensory Perception, 11, 95–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karim, A. A., Luetzenkirchen, B., Khedr, E. M., & Khalil, R. (2017). Why is 10 past 10 the default setting for clocks and watches in advertisements? A psychological experiment. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1410. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01410

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Karim, A., Proulx, M. J., & Likova, L. T. (2016). Anticlockwise or clockwise? A dynamic perception-action-laterality model for directionality bias in visuospatial functioning. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 669–693. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.032

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kawachi, Y., Kawabata, H., Kitamura, M. S., Shibata, M., Imaizumi, O., & Gyoba, J. (2011). Topographic distribution of brain activities corresponding to psychological structures underlying affective meanings: An fMRI study. Japanese Psychological Research, 53, 361–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenward, B., Wachtmeister, C.-A., Ghirlanda, S., & Enquist, M. (2004). Spots and stripes: The evolution of repetition in visual signal form. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 230, 407–419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovač, A., Kovačević, D., Bota, J., & Brozović, M. (2019). Consumers’ preferences for visual elements on chocolate packaging. Journal of Graphic Engineering and Design, 10, 13–18. https://doi.org/10.24867/JGED-2019-1-013

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krishna, A., Cian, L., & Aydinoğlu, N. Z. (2017). Sensory aspects of package design. Journal of Retailing, 93, 43–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroese, F. M., Marchiori, D. R., & de Ridder, D. T. D. (2016). Nudging healthy food choices: A field experiment at the train station. Journal of Public Health, 38, e133–e137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kühn, S., Brick, T. R., Müller, B. C. N., & Gallinat, J. (2014). Is this car looking at you? How anthropomorphism predicts fusiform face area activation when seeing cars. PLoS ONE, 9(12), e113885. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0113885

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kühn, S., Strelow, E., & Gallinat, J. (2016). Multiple “buy buttons” in the brain: Forecasting chocolate sales at point-of-sale based on functional brain activation using fMRI. NeuroImage, 136, 122–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kujala, S., & Nurkka, P. (2012). Sentence completion for evaluating symbolic meaning. International Journal of Design, 6(3), 15–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunz, S., Haasova, S., & Florack, A. (2020). Fifty shades of food: The influence of package color saturation on health and taste in consumer judgments. Psychology & Marketing, 37, 900–912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labrecque, L. L., & Milne, G. R. (2012). Exciting red and competent blue: The importance of color in marketing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 40, 711–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labrecque, L. L., & Milne, G. R. (2013). To be or not to be different: Exploration of norms and benefits of color differentiation in the marketplace. Marketing Letters, 24, 165–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labrecque, L. L., Patrick, V. M., & Milne, G. R. (2013). The marketer’s prismatic palette: A review of color research and future directions. Psychology & Marketing, 30, 187–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwartz, N. (2008). Of frog wines and frowning watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 819–831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lafontaine, M. P., Knoth, I. S., & Lippé, S. (2020). Chapter 20—Learning abilities. In A. Gallagher, C. Bulteau, D. Cohen, & J. L. Michaud (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology (Vol. 173, pp. 241–254). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-64150-2.00021-6

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., & Stearns, J. J. (2007). The shape of threat: Simple geometric forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. Emotion, 7, 526–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. L. (1959). Symbols for sale. Harvard Business Review, 37(4), 117–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, M., Jin, Y., Zhang, J., & Liu, R. (2022). Does shape in backgrounds matter? Effects of shape–taste congruence on product evaluations. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 67, 102990. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2022.102990

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieske, E., & Myers, R. (1994). Coral reef fishes: Indo-Pacific & Caribbean. HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake: The relative roles of perception, cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion, 14, 701–711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lunardo, R., & Livat, F. (2016). Congruency between color and shape of the front labels of wine: Effects on fluency and aroma and quality perceptions. International Journal of Entrepreneurship and Small Business, 29(4), 528–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lunardo, R., Saintives, C., & Chaney, D. (2021). Food packaging and the color red: How negative cognitive associations influence feelings of guilt. Journal of Business Research, 134, 589–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lunt, S. G. (1981). Using focus groups in packaging research. In W. Stern (Ed.), Handbook of package design research (pp. 112–124). Wiley Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Machiels, C. J., & Orth, U. R. (2017). Verticality in product labels and shelves as a metaphorical cue to quality. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 37, 195–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mai, R., Symmank, C., & Seeberg-Elverfeldt, B. (2016). Light and pale colors in food packaging: When does this package cue signal superior healthiness or inferior tastiness? Journal of Retailing, 92, 426–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marion. (2015). What to learn from Tropicana’s packaging redesign failure? The Branding Journal, May 1st. https://www.thebrandingjournal.com/2015/05/what-to-learn-from-tropicanas-packaging-redesign-failure/

  • Marques da Rosa, V., Spence, C., & Tonetto, M. L. (2019). Influences of visual attributes of food packaging on consumer preference and associations with taste and healthiness. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 43, 210–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, P., Simmonds, G., & Spence, C. (2019). Establishing boundary conditions for multiple design elements congruent with taste expectations. Food Quality & Preference, 78, 103742. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103742

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, J. A., Richerson, R., & Li, W. (2020). Dynamic right-slanted fonts increase the effectiveness of promotional retail advertising. Journal of Retailing, 96(2), 284–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merlo, T., Soletti, I., Saldaña, E., Menegali, B., Martins, M., Teixeira, A., Harada-Padermo, S., Dargelio, M., & Contreras-Castillo, C. (2018). Measuring dynamics of emotions evoked by the packaging colour of hamburgers using Temporal Dominance of Emotions (TDE). Food Research International, 124, 147–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2018.08.007

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyers, H. M. (1981). Determining communication objectives for package design. In W. Stern (Ed.), Handbook of package design research (pp. 22–38). Wiley Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mick, G. D. (1986). Consumer research and semiotics: Exploring the morphology of signs, symbols, and significance. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 196–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirabito, A., Oliphant, M., Van Doorn, G., Watson, S., & Spence, C. (2017). Glass shape influences the flavour of beer. Food Quality and Preference, 62, 257–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morich, D. (1981). Using tachistoscope, semantic differential and preference tests in package design assessment. In W. Stern (Ed.), Handbook of package design research (pp. 125–140). Wiley Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motoki, K., & Velasco, C. (2021). Taste-shape correspondences in context. Food Quality and Preference, 88, 104082. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104082

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moya, I., García-Madariaga, J., & Blasco, M.-F. (2020). What can neuromarketing tell us about food packaging? Foods, 9(12), 1856. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9121856

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mukherjee, K., Yin, B., Sherman, B. E., Lessard, L., & Schloss, K. B. (2022). Context matters: A theory of semantic discriminability for perceptual encoding systems. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 28(1), 697–706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ngo, M. K., Velasco, C., Salgado, A., Boehm, E., O’Neill, D., & Spence, C. (2013). Assessing crossmodal correspondences in exotic fruit juices: The case of shape and sound symbolism. Food Quality & Preference, 28, 361–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obrist, M., Comber, R., Subramanian, S., Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (2014). Temporal, affective, and embodied characteristics of taste experiences. In Proceedings of the 32nd annual ACM conference on human factors in computing systems—CHI ’14 (pp. 2853–2862). ACM Press. https://doi.org/10.1145/2556288.2557007521

  • Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overbeeke, C. J., & Peters, M. E. (1991). The taste of desserts’ packages. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 73, 575–580.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, T. (2003). Affective and symbolic meanings of color and form: Experimental psychological approaches. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 21(2), 137–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, T., Yamada, H., & Iwasawa, H. (1998). Synesthetic tendencies as the basis of sensory symbolism: A review of a series of experiments by means of semantic differential. Psychologia, 41(3), 203–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parise, C. V., & Spence, C. (2012). Assessing the associations between brand packaging and brand attributes using an indirect performance measure. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 17–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.08.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J., Spence, C., Ishii, H., & Togawa, T. (2021). Turning the other cheek: Facial orientation influences both model attractiveness and product evaluation. Psychology & Marketing, 38(1), 7–20. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21398

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pazda, A. D., Elliot, A. J., & Greitemeyer, T. (2011). Sexy red: Perceived sexual receptivity mediates the red-attraction relation in men viewing woman. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 787–790. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pecchinenda, A., Bertamini, M., Makin, A. D. J., & Ruta, N. (2014). The pleasantness of visual symmetry: Always, never or sometimes. PLoS ONE, 9(3), e92685.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Peng-Li, D., Byrne, D. V., Chan, R. C. K., & Wang, Q. J. (2020). The influence of taste-congruent soundtracks on visual attention and food choice: A cross-cultural eye-tracking study in Chinese and Danish consumers. Food Quality and Preference, 85, 103692. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103962

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The child’s conception of number. Basic Books (original in French, 1941).

  • Piqueras-Fiszman, B., & Spence, C. (2015). Sensory expectations based on product-extrinsic food cues: An interdisciplinary review of the empirical evidence and theoretical accounts. Food Quality & Preference, 40, 165–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.09.013

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Velasco, C., Salgado-Montejo, A., & Spence, C. (2013). Combined eye tracking and word association analysis to evaluate the impact of changing the multisensory attributes of food packaging. Food Quality & Preference, 28, 328–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (2012). Exploring implicit and explicit crossmodal colour-flavour correspondences in product packaging. Food Quality & Preference, 25, 148–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plasschaert, J. (1995). The meaning of colour on packaging—A methodology for qualitative research using semiotic principles and computer image manipulation. In Decision making and research in action. 48th ESOMAR Marketing Research Congress (pp. 217–232). Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

  • Pombo, M., & Velasco, C. (2021). How aesthetic features convey the concept of brand premiumness. Psychology & Marketing, 38(9), 1475–1497. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21534

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, G. W. (1994). The contour: A packaging vision seen through Coke-bottle lenses. Beverage World, 113(Periscope Edition), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raghubir, P., & Greenleaf, E. (2006). Ratios in proportion: What should the shape of the package be? Journal of Marketing, 70(April), 95–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raghubir, P., & Krishna, A. (1999). Vital dimensions in volume perception: Can the eye fool the stomach? Journal of Marketing Research, 36, 313–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapaille, C. (2007). The culture code. Crown Business.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rebollar, R., Lidón, I., Serrano, A., Martín, J., & Fernández, M. J. (2012). Influence of chewing gum packaging design on consumer expectation and willingness to buy. An analysis of functional, sensory and experience attributes. Food Quality & Preference, 24, 162–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinoso-Carvalho, F., Campo, R., De Luca, M., & Velasco, C. (2021). Toward healthier cookie habits: Assessing the role of packaging visual appearance in the expectations for dietary cookies in digital environments. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 679443. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.679443

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reutskaja, E., Nagel, R., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2011). Search dynamics in consumer choice under time pressure: An eye-tracking study. The American Economic Review, 101(2), 900–926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodionova, Z. (19 July, 2016). Tesco and other supermarkets using fake farm brands spark complaint from NFU. Accessed June 30, 2021 from https://www.independent.co.uk/news/business/news/tesco-and-other-supermarkets-using-fake-farm-brands-spark-complaint-from-nfu-a7144551.html

  • Romero, M., & Biswas, D. (2016). Healthy-left, unhealthy-right: Can displaying healthy items to the left (versus right) of unhealthy items nudge healthier choices? Journal of Consumer Research, 43(1), 103–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rorink, J. M. (2018). Vertical limits: The effects of verticality cues and a quality claim on consumer responses to coffee ad-displays [Unpublished Master’s Thesis]. University of Twente.

  • Rundh, B. (2005). The multi-faceted dimension of packaging. British Food Journal, 107, 670–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salgado-Montejo, A., Alvarado, J., Velasco, C., Salgado, C. J., Hasse, K., & Spence, C. (2015a). The sweetest thing: The influence of angularity, symmetry, and the number of elements on shape-valence and shape-taste matches. Frontiers in Psychology: Perception Science, 6, 1382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salgado-Montejo, A., Tapia Leon, I., Elliot, A. J., Salgado, C. J., & Spence, C. (2015b). Smiles over frowns: When curved lines influence product preference. Psychology & Marketing, 32, 771–781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salgado-Montejo, A., Velasco, C., Olier, J. S., Alvarado, J., & Spence, C. (2014). Love for logos: Evaluating the congruency between brand symbols and typefaces and their relation to emotional words. Journal of Brand Management, 21, 635–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuel, L. R. (2010). Freud on Madison Avenue: Motivation research and subliminal advertising in America. University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schaefer, M., & Rotte, M. (2010). Combining a semantic differential with fMRI to investigate brands as cultural symbols. Scan, 5, 274–281.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2001). Effects of product beliefs on product perception and liking. In L. Frewer, E. Risvik, & H. Schifferstein (Eds.), Food, people and society: A European perspective of consumers’ food choices (pp. 73–96). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schifferstein, H. N. J., Fenko, A., Desmet, P. M. A., Labbe, D., & Martin, N. (2013). Influence of packaging design on the dynamics of multisensory and emotional food experience. Food Quality & Preference, 27, 18–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schifferstein, H. N. J., & Howell, B. F. (2015). Using color-odor correspondences for fragrance packaging design. Food Quality & Preference, 46, 17–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schifferstein, H. N. J., & Verlegh, P. W. J. (1996). The role of congruency and pleasantness in odor-induced taste enhancement. Acta Psychologica, 94, 87–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schloss, K. B., Gramazio, C. C., Silverman, A. T., Parker, M. L., & Wang, A. S. (2019). Mapping color to meaning in colormap data visualizations. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 25(1), 810–819.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schloss, K. B., Leggon, Z., & Lessard, L. (2021). Semantic discriminability for visual communication. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 27(2), 1022–1031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schloss, K. B., Lessard, L., Walmsley, C. S., & Foley, K. (2018). Color inference in visual communication: The meaning of colors in recycling. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3, 5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, T. W. (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(1), 1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuldt, J. P. (2013). Does green mean healthy? Nutrition label color affects perceptions of healthfulness. Health Communication, 28, 814–821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seriously Science. (21 April, 2014). Wear what you want: Scientific proof that horizontal stripes don't make you look fatter. Accessed June 22, 2021 from https://www.discovermagazine.com/the-sciences/wear-what-you-want-scientific-proof-that-horizontal-stripes-dont-make-you-look-fatter

  • Shen, X., Wan, X., Mu, B., & Spence, C. (2015). Searching for triangles: An extension to food & packaging. Food Quality & Preference, 44, 26–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silayoi, P., & Speece, M. (2007). The importance of packaging attributes: A conjoint analysis approach. European Journal of Marketing, 41(11–12), 1495–1517. https://doi.org/10.1108/03090560710821279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmonds, G., & Spence, C. (2017). Thinking inside the box: Can seeing products on or through the packaging influence consumer purchase behaviour? Food Quality & Preference, 62, 340–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmonds, G., & Spence, C. (2019). Food imagery and transparency in product packaging. In C. Velasco & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory packaging: Designing new product experiences (pp. 49–77). Palgrave MacMillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Simmonds, G., Woods, A., & Spence, C. (2018a). “Show me the goods”: Assessing the effectiveness of transparent packaging vs. product imagery. Food Quality and Preference, 63, 18–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmonds, G., Woods, A., & Spence, C. (2018b). “Seeing what’s left”: The effect of position of transparent windows on product evaluations. Foods, 7, 151. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods7090151

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Simmonds, G., Woods, A., & Spence, C. (2019). ‘Shaping perceptions’: Exploring how the shape of transparent windows in packaging designs affects product evaluation. Food Quality and Preference, 75, 15–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.02.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, S. (2006). Impact of colour on marketing. Management Decision, 44(6), 783–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skaczkowski, G., Durkin, S., Kashima, Y., & Wakefield, M. (2016). The effect of packaging, branding and labeling on the experience of unhealthy food and drink: A review. Appetite, 99, 219–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snider, J. G., & Osgood, C. E. (1969). Semantic differential technique: A sourcebook. Aldine Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Söderlund, M., Colliander, J., Karsberg, J., Liljedal, K. L., Modig, E., Rosengren, S., Sagfossen, S., Szugalski, S., & Åkestam, N. (2017). The allure of the bottle as a package: An assessment of perceived effort in a packaging context. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 26(1), 91–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 971–995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (2012). Managing sensory expectations concerning products and brands: Capitalizing on the potential of sound and shape symbolism. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22, 37–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (appearing under name Willis, S.) (2019a). Brand illusions: The subliminal message of San Pellegrino’s label: Why stars make your water sparkle. The Economist 1843 Magazine, April/May. http://www.1843magazine.com/design/brand-illusions/why-stars-make-your-water-sparkle.

  • Spence, C. (2019b). Tactile/haptic aspects of multisensory packaging design. In C. Velasco & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory packaging: Designing new product experiences (pp. 127–159). Palgrave MacMillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (2020a). Olfactory-colour crossmodal correspondences in art, science, & design. Cognitive Research: Principles & Implications: CRPI, 5, 52. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-020-00246-1

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (2020b). Temperature-based crossmodal correspondences: Causes & consequences. Multisensory Research, 33, 645–682. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134808-20191494

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (2021a). Sensehacking: How to use the power of your senses for happier, heathier living. Viking Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (2021b). Glossy packaging: On the questionable appeal of glossy/shiny food packaging. Foods, 10, 959. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods10050959

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (2021c). On the multisensory design of pharmaceuticals and their packaging. Food Quality & Preference, 91, 104200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C. (2021d). What’s the story with blue steak? On the unexpected popularity of blue foods. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 638703. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.638703

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., & Deroy, O. (2012). On the shapes of tastes and flavours. Petits Propos Culinaires, 97, 75–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., & Deroy, O. (2013). Tasting shapes: A review of four hypotheses. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum, 10, 207–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., & Levitan, C. A. (2022). Exploring the links between colours and tastes/flavours. Journal of Perceptual Imaging (JPI), 4(3), 000408. https://doi.org/10.2352/J.Percept.Imaging.2022.5.000408

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., Michel, C., Youssef, J., & Woods, A. (2019). Assessing the aesthetic oblique effect in painting and plating. International Journal of Gastronomy & Food Science, 17, 100168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgfs.2019.100168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., & Parise, C. V. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of crossmodal correspondences. i-Perception, 3(7), 410–412. https://doi.org/10.1068/i0540ic

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., & Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2012). The multisensory packaging of beverages. In M. G. Kontominas (Ed.), Food packaging: Procedures, management and trends (pp. 187–233). Nova Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., Smith, B., & Auvray, M. (2015a). Confusing tastes and flavours. In D. Stokes, M. Matthen, & S. Biggs (Eds.), Perception and its modalities (pp. 247–274). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., & Van Doorn, G. H. (2017). Does the shape of the drinking receptacle influence taste/flavour perception? A review. Beverages, 3(3), 33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., & Velasco, C. (2018). On the multiple effects of packaging colour on consumer behaviour and product experience in the ‘food and beverage’ and ‘home and personal care’ categories. Food Quality & Preference, 68, 226–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., & Velasco, C. (2019). Packaging colour and its multiple roles. In C. Velasco & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory packaging: Designing new product experiences (pp. 21–48). Palgrave MacMillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., Wan, X., Woods, A., Velasco, C., Deng, J., Youssef, J., & Deroy, O. (2015b). On tasty colours and colourful tastes? Assessing, explaining, and utilizing crossmodal correspondences between colours and basic tastes. Flavour, 4, 23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stelzer, R. J., Raine, N. E., Schmitt, K. D., & Chittka, L. (2010). Effects of aposematic coloration on predation risk in bumblebees? A comparison between differently coloured populations, with consideration of the ultraviolet. Journal of Zoology, 282, 75–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2010.00709.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, W. (Ed.). (1981). Handbook of package design research. Wiley Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sugermeyer, K. (2021). From conscious to non-conscious: Understanding the role of packaging design. NielsenIQ, October 29th. https://nielseniq.com/global/en/insights/education/2021/from-conscious-to-non-conscious-understanding-the-role-of-packaging-design/

  • Sumner, P., & Mollon, J. D. (2003). Did primate trichromacy evolve for frugivory or folivory? In J. D. Mollon, J. Pokorny, & K. Knoblauch (Eds.), Normal and defective colour vision (pp. 21–30). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198525301.003.0003

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sunaga, T., Park, J., & Spence, C. (2016). Effects of lightness-location consumers’ purchase decision-making. Psychology & Marketing, 33, 934–950.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sundar, A., & Noseworthy, T. J. (2014). Place the logo high or low? Using conceptual metaphors of power in packaging design. Journal of Marketing, 78(5), 138–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sundar, A., & Noseworthy, T. J. (2016). When sensory marketing works and when it backfires. Harvard Business Review, May 19th. https://hbr.org/2016/05/when-sensory-marketing-works-and-when-it-backfires?referral=03758&cm_vc=rr_item_page.top_right

  • Suzuki, M., Gyoba, J., & Sakuta, Y. (2005). Multichannel NIRS analysis of brain activity during semantic differential rating of drawing stimuli containing different affective polarities. Neuroscience Letters, 375, 53–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tham, D. S. Y., Sowden, P. T., Grandison, A., Franklin, A., Lee, A. K. W., Ng, M., Park, J., Pang, W., & Zhao, J. (2020). A systematic investigation of conceptual color associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(7), 1311–1332. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000703

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theben, A., Gerards, M., & Folkvord, F. (2020). The effect of packaging color and health claims on product attitude and buying intention. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(6), 1991. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17061991

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P., & Mikellidou, K. (2009). The 3-D Helmholtz square illusion: More reasons to wear horizontal stripes. Journal of Vision, 9(8), 50. https://doi.org/10.1167/9.8.50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, D. M. H. (2016). Sensory branding: Using brand, pack, and product sensory characteristics to deliver a compelling brand message. In B. Piqueras-Fiszman & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory flavor perception: From fundamental neuroscience through to the marketplace (pp. 313–336). Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tijssen, I., Zandstra, E. H., de Graaf, C., & Jager, G. (2017). Why a ‘light’ product package should not be light blue: Effects of package colour on perceived healthiness and attractiveness of sugar- and fat-reduced products. Food Quality and Preference, 59, 46–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Togawa, T., Park, J., Ishii, H., & Deng, X. (2019). A packaging visual-gustatory correspondence effect: Using visual packaging design to influence flavor perception and healthy eating decisions. Journal of Retailing, 95(4), 204–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tom, G., Barnett, T., Lew, W., & Selmants, J. (1987). Cueing the consumer: The role of salient cues in consumer perception. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 4(2), 23–27. https://doi.org/10.1108/eb008193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turoman, N., Velasco, C., Chen, Y.-C., Huang, P.-C., & Spence, C. (2018). Tasting transformations: Symmetry and its role in the crossmodal correspondence between shape and taste. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 738–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, R. L. (1993). Packaging as an extrinsic product attribute: An examination of package utility and its effect on total product utility in a consumer purchase situation. In R. Varadarajan & B. Jaworski (Eds.), Marketing theory and applications (Vol. 4, pp. 212–217). American Marketing Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, R. L. (1999). Construction of identity through packaging: A qualitative inquiry. In A. Menon & A. Sharma (Eds.), Marketing theory and applications (Vol. 10, p. 147).

  • Underwood, R. L., Klein, N., & Burke, R. (2001). Packaging communication: Attentional effects of product imagery. Journal of Product and Brand Management, 10, 403–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, R. L., & Ozanne, J. (1998). Is your package an effective communicator? A normative framework for increasing the communicative competence of packaging. Journal of Marketing Communication, 4, 207–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, S., & Klein, N. (2002). Packaging as brand communication: Effects of product pictures on consumer responses to the package and brand. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 10(4), 58–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valinsky, J. (November 18, 2020). This is what the KFC of the future will look like. Accessed June 18, 2021 from https://edition.cnn.com/2020/11/18/business/kfc-restaurant-redesign/index.html

  • Van Den Berg-Weitzel, L., & Van Den Laar, G. (2001). Relation between culture and communication in packaging design. Journal of Brand Management, 8(3), 171–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Doorn, G., Woods, A., Levitan, C. A., Wan, X., Velasco, C., Bernal-Torres, C., & Spence, C. (2017). Does the shape of a cup influence coffee taste expectations? A cross-cultural, online study. Food Quality & Preference, 56, 201–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Doorn, G., Wuillemin, D., & Spence, C. (2014). Does the colour of the mug influence the taste of the coffee? Flavour, 3, 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/2044-7248-3-10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Esch, P., Heller, J., & Northey, G. (2019). The effects of inner packaging color on the desirability of food. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 50, 94–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2019.05.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Lee, Q., et al. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110(47), 19000-19005. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1312648110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Ooijen, I., Fransen, M. L., Verlegh, P. W. J., & Smit, E. G. (2016). Atypical food packaging affects the persuasive impact of product claims. Food Quality and Preference, 48, 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.08.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Rompay, T. J. L., de Vries, P. W., Bontekoe, F., & Tanja-Dijkstra, K. (2012). Embodied product perception: Effects of verticality cues in advertising and packaging design on consumer impressions and price expectations. Psychology & Marketing, 29, 919–928.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Rompay, T., Deterink, F., & Fenko, A. (2016). Healthy package, healthy product? Effects of packaging design as a function of purchase setting. Food Quality & Preference, 43, 84–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Rompay, T. J. L., van Hoof, J. J., Rorink, J., & Folsche, M. (2019). Served straight up: Effects of verticality cues on taste evaluations and luxury perceptions. Appetite, 135, 72–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.01.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Adams, C., Petit, O., & Spence, C. (2019a). On the localization of tastes and tasty products in 2D space. Food Quality & Preference, 71, 438–446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.08.018

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Hyndman, S., & Spence, C. (2018a). The role of typeface curvilinearity on taste expectations and perception. International Journal of Gastronomy & Food Science, 11, 63–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Michel, C., Youssef, J., Gomez, X., Cheok, A. D., & Spence, C. (2016a). Colour-taste correspondences: Design food experiences to meet expectations or surprise. International Journal of Food Design, 1, 83–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Salgado-Montejo, A., Marmolejo-Ramos, F., & Spence, C. (2014a). Predictive packaging design: Tasting shapes, typographies, names, and sounds. Food Quality & Preference, 34, 88–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (2019b). The role of typeface in packaging design. In C. Velasco & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory packaging: Designing new product experiences (pp. 79–101). Palgrave MacMillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (Eds.). (2019a). Multisensory packaging: Designing new product experiences. Palgrave MacMillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-94977-2

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (2019c). Multisensory premiumness. In C. Velasco & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory packaging: Designing new product experiences (pp. 257–286). Palgrave MacMillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Wan, X., Knoeferle, K., Zhou, X., Salgado-Montejo, A., & Spence, C. (2015a). Searching for flavour labels in food products: The influence of color-flavor congruence and association strength. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 301. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00301

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Wan, X., Salgado-Montejo, A., Woods, A., Andrés Oñate, G., Mu, B., & Spence, C. (2014b). The context of colour-flavour associations in crisps packaging: A cross-cultural study comparing Chinese, Colombian, and British consumers. Food Quality & Preference, 38, 49–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Woods, A. T., Hyndman, S., & Spence, C. (2015b). The taste of typeface. i-Perception, 6(4), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Woods, A. T., Petit, O., Cheok, A. D., & Spence, C. (2016b). Crossmodal correspondences between taste and shape, and their implications for product packaging: A review. Food Quality and Preference, 52, 17–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Woods, A. T., & Spence, C. (2015c). Evaluating the orientation of design elements in product packaging using an online orientation task. Food Quality & Preference, 46, 151–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., Woods, A. T., Wan, X., Salgado-Montejo, A., Bernal-Torres, C. A., Cheok, A. D., & Spence, C. (2018b). The taste of typefaces in different countries and languages. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 236–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeir, I., & Roose, G. (2020). Visual design cues impacting food choice: A review and future research agenda. Foods, 9, 1495. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9101495

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Visser, E. (2009). Packaging design: A cultural sign. Index Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagemans, J. (Ed.). (2015). The Oxford handbook of perceptual organization. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, P., & Walker, L. (2012). Size-brightness correspondence: Crosstalk and congruity among dimensions of connotative meaning. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1226–1240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, E. (2013). The influence of visual packaging design on perceived food product quality, value, and brand preference. International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 41(10), 805–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, F., & Basso, F. (2019). “Animals are friends, not food”: Anthropomorphism leads to less favourable attitudes toward meat consumption by inducing feelings of anticipatory guilt. Appetite, 138, 153–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.03.019

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, F., & Basso, F. (2021). The peak of health: The vertical representation of healthy food. Appetite, 167, 105587. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2021.105587

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., Yu, Y., & Li, O. (2020). The typeface curvature effect: The role of typeface curvature in increasing preference toward hedonic products. Psychology & Marketing, 37, 1118–1137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, R. W., & Sun, C. H. (2006). Analysis of interrelations between bottle shape and food taste. Paper presented at the 2006 Design Research Society, International Conference in Lisbon (IADE) (p. 13). http://www.iade.pt/drs2006/wonderground/proceedings/fullpapers/DRS2006_0054.pdf

  • Watson, D. G., Blagrove, E., Evans, C., & Moore, L. (2011). Negative triangles: Simple geometric shapes convey emotional valence. Emotion, 12, 18–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024495

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, S. (1981). Brain wave analysis: The beginning and future of package design research. In W. Stern (Ed.), Handbook of package design research (pp. 492–504). Wiley Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheatley, J. (1973). Putting colour into marketing. Marketing, 67(October), 24–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windhager, S., Slice, D. E., Schaefer, K., Oberzaucher, E., Thorstensen, T., & Grammer, K. (2008). Face to face: The perception of automotive designs. Human Nature, 19, 331–346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, A. T., Marmolejo-Ramos, F., Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (2016). Using single colours and colour pairs to communicate basic tastes II; Foreground-background colour combinations. i-Perception, 7, 5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, A. T., & Spence, C. (2016). Using single colours and colour pairs to communicate basic tastes. i-Perception, 7, 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, A. T., Spence, C., Butcher, N., & Deroy, O. (2013). Fast lemons and sour boulders: Testing crossmodal correspondences using an internet-based testing methodology. i-Perception, 4, 365–369.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, A. T., Velasco, C., Levitan, C. A., Wan, X., & Spence, C. (2015). Conducting perception research over the internet: A tutorial review. PeerJ, 3, 1058. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1058

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yarar, N., Machiels, C. J. A., & Orth, U. R. (2019). Shaping up: How package shape and consumer body conspire to affect food healthiness evaluation. Food Quality & Preference, 75, 209–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yates, J. L. (June 1, 2021). 27 brands supporting LGBTQ pride in style. Accessed November 9, 2021 from https://www.goodmorningamerica.com/style/story/17-brands-supporting-lgbtq-pride-style-77817097

  • Zhao, H., Huang, F., Spence, C., & Wan, X. (2017). Visual search for wines with a triangle on the label in a virtual store. Frontiers in Psychology: Human-Media Interaction, 8, 2173. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, H., Qi, Y., Spence, C., & Wan, X. (2020). On the costs and benefits of using triangles in packaging design. Food Quality & Preference, 78, 103719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103719

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

None.

Significance statement

The visual attributes of product packaging play a key role in terms of communicating with the consumer, not to mention capturing their attention while the packaging is displayed on the shelf or online. Both semantically meaningful and abstract visual design elements combine to convey meaning/prime associations in the mind of the consumer. While traditionally, decisions about visual design were often made intuitively or on the basis of focus groups or in-depth interviews, there has been a recent growth of scientific interest in understanding the way(s) in which various abstract design elements communicate with the consumer. This narrative review summarizes the various theoretical accounts that have been put forward to help explain the meaning of colour, shape, texture, and stripes in product packaging, including accounts in terms of crossmodal correspondences, connotative meaning, symbolic meaning, semantic meaning, and evolutionary accounts. While the primary focus of this review is on using abstract visual design features to communicate taste properties, the signalling of other attributes such as variant, brand, quality, natural/healthy, and price is also discussed where relevant. Several directions for future research that should help determine the likely meaning abstract visual design cues when used in combination are also outlined.

Funding

Completion of this review was supported by AHRC ‘Rethinking the Senses’ Grant AH/L007053/1.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles Spence.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

The authors confirm that they have consent to publish this work.

Competing interests

There are no competing interests to declare.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Spence, C., Van Doorn, G. Visual communication via the design of food and beverage packaging. Cogn. Research 7, 42 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-022-00391-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-022-00391-9

Keywords