Åkerstedt, T., & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. International Journal of Neuroscience, 52, 29–37. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459008994241.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Arnold, N. R., & Bayen, U. J. (2019). Prospective memory: Comparing self- and proxy-reports with cognitive modeling of task performance. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8, 244–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.04.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Arnold, N. R., Bayen, U. J., & Böhm, M. F. (2015). Is prospective memory related to depression and anxiety? A hierarchical MPT modelling approach. Memory, 23, 1215–1228. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.969276.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Arnold, N. R., Bayen, U. J., & Smith, R. E. (2015). Hierarchical multinomial modeling approaches: An application to prospective memory and working memory. Experimental Psychology, 62, 143–152. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000287.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Axelsson, J., Kecklund, G., Åkerstedt, T., Donofrio, P., Lekander, M., & Ingre, M. (2008). Sleepiness and performance in response to repeated sleep restriction and subsequent recovery during semi-laboratory conditions. Chronobiology International, 25, 297–308. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420520802107031.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Backhaus, J., Junghanns, K., Broocks, A., Riemann, D., & Hohagen, F. (2002). Test–retest reliability and validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index in primary insomnia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 737–740. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00330-6.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Barner, C., Seibold, M., Born, J., & Diekelmann, S. (2016). Consolidation of prospective memory: Effects of sleep on completed and reinstated intentions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 2025. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02025.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1999). Theoretical and empirical review of multinomial process tree modeling. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 57–86. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210812.
Article
Google Scholar
Benitez, A., & Gunstad, J. (2012). Poor sleep quality diminishes cognitive functioning independent of depression and anxiety in healthy young adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26, 214–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2012.658439.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bezdicek, O., Nikolai, T., Nepožitek, J., Peřinová, P., Kemlink, D., Dušek, P., et al. (2018). Prospective memory impairment in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 32, 1019–1037. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2017.1394493.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bleich, S., Havemann-Reinecke, U., & Kornhuber, J. (2002). FTNA: Fagerström-Test für Nikotinabhängigkeit [FTNA: Fagerström-Test for nicotine dependency]. Weinheim: Beltz.
Google Scholar
Bloch, K. E., Schoch, O. D., Zhang, J. N., & Russi, E. W. (1999). German Version of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Respiration, 66(5), 440–447. https://doi.org/10.1159/000029408.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bonnet, M. H. (1985). Effect of sleep disruption on sleep, performance, and mood. Sleep, 8, 11–19. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/8.1.11.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Borbély, A. A. (1982). A two process model of sleep regulation. Human Neurobiology, 1, 195–204.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., & Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28, 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Byun, E., Kim, J., & Riegel, B. (2017). Associations of subjective sleep quality and daytime sleepiness with cognitive impairment in adults and elders with heart failure. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 15, 302–317. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2015.1133418.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Caldwell, J. A., Prazinko, B., & Caldwell, J. L. (2003). Body posture affects electroencephalographic activity and psychomotor vigilance task performance in sleep-deprived subjects. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00283-3.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Casey, S. J., Solomons, L. C., Steier, J., Kabra, N., Burnside, A., Pengo, M. F., et al. (2016). Slow wave and REM sleep deprivation effects on explicit and implicit memory during sleep. Neuropsychology, 30, 931–945. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000314.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cavuoto, M. G., Ong, B., Pike, K. E., Nicholas, C. L., Bei, B., & Kinsella, G. J. (2016). Objective but not subjective sleep predicts memory in community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Sleep Research, 25, 475–485. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12391.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2008). Perceived insufficient rest or sleep - four states, 2006. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 57, 200–203 https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5708a2.htm.
Google Scholar
Chau, J. Y., Grunseit, A. C., Chey, T., Stamatakis, E., Brown, W. J., Matthews, C. E., et al. (2013). Daily sitting time and all-cause mortality: A meta-analysis. PlOS One, 8(11), e80000. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080000.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Cohrs, S., Rodenbeck, A., Riemann, D., Szagun, B., Jaehne, A., Brinkmeyer, J., et al. (2014). Impaired sleep quality and sleep duration in smokers: Results from the German Multicenter Study on Nicotine Dependence. Addiction Biology, 19, 486–496. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00487.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cole, R. J. (1989). Postural baroreflex stimuli may affect EEG arousal and sleep in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology, 67, 2367–2375. https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1989.67.6.2369.
Article
Google Scholar
Cook, C., Ball, B. H., & Brewer, G. A. (2014). No effects of executive control depletion on prospective memory retrieval processes. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 121–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.04.006.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cousins, J. N., Sasmita, K., & Chee, M. W. L. (2018). Memory encoding is impaired after multiple nights of partial sleep restriction. Journal of Sleep Research, 27, 138–145. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12578.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Diekelmann, S., Wilhelm, I., Wagner, U., & Born, J. (2013a). Sleep improves prospective remembering by facilitating spontaneous-associative retrieval processes. PlOS One, 8, e77621. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077621.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Diekelmann, S., Wilhelm, I., Wagner, U., & Born, J. (2013b). Sleep to implement an intention. Sleep, 36, 149–153. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.2322.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Dinges, D. F., & Powell, J. W. (1985). Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 17, 652–655. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200977.
Article
Google Scholar
Dismukes, R. K. (2012). Prospective memory in workplace and everyday situations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 215–220. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412447621.
Article
Google Scholar
Drummond, S. P. A., Gillin, J. C., & Brown, G. G. (2001). Increased cerebral response during a divided attention task following sleep deprivation. Journal of Sleep Research, 10, 85–92. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00245.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 717–726. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Erdfelder, E., Auer, T.-S., Hilbig, B. E., Aßfalg, A., Moshagen, M., & Nadarevic, L. (2009). Multinomial processing tree models: A review of the literature. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 108–124. https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409.217.3.108.
Article
Google Scholar
Esposito, M. J., Occhionero, M., & Cicogna, P. (2015). Sleep deprivation and time-based prospective memory. Sleep, 38, 1823–1826. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.5172.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fabbri, M., Tonetti, L., Martoni, M., & Natale, V. (2014). Sleep and prospective memory. Biological Rhythm Research, 45, 115–120. https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2013.830510.
Article
Google Scholar
Fabbri, M., Tonetti, L., Martoni, M., & Natale, V. (2015). Remember to do: Insomnia versus control groups in a prospective memory task. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 13, 231–240. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2013.860896.
Article
Google Scholar
Gelman, A., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Statistical Science, 7, 457–472. https://doi.org/10.1214/ss/1177011136.
Article
Google Scholar
Gillberg, M., Kecklund, G., & Åkerstedt, T. (1994). Relations between performance and subjective ratings of sleepiness during a night awake. Sleep, 17, 236–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/17.3.236.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gobin, C. M., Banks, J. B., Fins, A. I., & Tartar, J. L. (2015). Poor sleep quality is associated with a negative cognitive bias and decreased sustained attention. Journal of Sleep Research, 24, 535–542. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12302.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gorgoni, M., Ferlazzo, F., Ferrara, M., Moroni, F., D'Atri, A., Fanelli, S., et al. (2014). Topographic electroencephalogram changes associated with psychomotor vigilance task performance after sleep deprivation. Sleep Medicine, 15, 1132–1139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2014.04.022.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Grundgeiger, T., Bayen, U. J., & Horn, S. S. (2014). Effects of sleep deprivation on prospective memory. Memory, 22, 679–686. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.812220.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hagen, E. M., Svensen, E., Eriksen, H. R., Ihlebaek, C. M., & Ursin, H. (2006). Comorbid subjective health complaints in low back pain. Spine, 31, 1491–1495. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.brs.0000219947.71168.08.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Heck, D. W., Arnold, N. R., & Arnold, D. (2018). TreeBUGS: An R package for hierarchical multinomial-processing-tree modeling. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 264–284. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0869-7.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Heister, J., Würzner, K.-M., Bubenzer, J., Pohl, E., Hanneforth, T., Geyken, A., & Kliegl, R. (2011). dlexDB – eine lexikalische Datenbank für die psychologische und linguistische Forschung [dlexDB – a lexical database for psychological and linguistical research]. Psychologische Rundschau, 62, 10–20. https://doi.org/10.1026/0033-3042/a000029.
Article
Google Scholar
Horn, S. S., Bayen, U. J., Smith, R. E., & Boywitt, C. D. (2011). The multinomial model of prospective memory: Validity of ongoing-task parameters. Experimental Psychology, 58, 247–255. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000091.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Horne, J. A., & Östberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4, 97–110.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ishihara, S. (1972). Tests for colour blindness. Tokyo: Kanehara Shuppan.
Google Scholar
Jaehne, A., Unbehaun, T., Feige, B., Lutz, U. C., Batra, A., & Riemann, D. (2012). How smoking affects sleep: A polysomnographical analysis. Sleep Medicine, 13, 1286–1292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2012.06.026.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Jewett, M. E., Dijk, D. J., Kronauer, R. E., & Dinges, D. F. (1999). Dose-response relationship between sleep duration and human psychomotor vigilance and subjective alertness. Sleep, 22, 171–179. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/22.2.171.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Johns, M. W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep, 14, 540–545. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/14.6.540.
Article
Google Scholar
Johns, M. W. (2000). Sensitivity and specificity of the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT), the maintenance of wakefulness test and the Epworth Sleepiness Scale: Failure of the MSLT as a gold standard. Journal of Sleep Research, 9, 5–11. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00177.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kaida, K., Åkerstedt, T., Kecklund, G., Nilsson, J. P., & Axelsson, J. (2007). Use of subjective and physiological indicators of sleepiness to predict performance during a vigilance task. Industrial Health, 45, 520–526. https://doi.org/10.2486/indhealth.45.520.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kaida, K., Takahashi, M., Åkerstedt, T., Nakata, A., Otsuka, Y., Haratani, T., & Fukasawa, K. (2006). Validation of the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale against performance and EEG variables. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117, 1574–1581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.03.011.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Klauer, K. C. (2010). Hierarchical multinomial processing tree models: A latent-trait approach. Psychometrika, 75, 70–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-009-9141-0.
Article
Google Scholar
Kräuchi, K., Cajochen, C., & Wirz-Justice, A. (1997). A relationship between heat loss and sleepiness: Effects of postural change and melatonin administration. Journal of Applied Physiology, 83, 134–139. https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1997.83.1.134.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kronholm, E., Partonen, T., Laatikainen, T., Peltonen, M., Härmä, M., Hublin, C., et al. (2008). Trends in self-reported sleep duration and insomnia-related symptoms in Finland from 1972 to 2005: A comparative review and re-analysis of Finnish population samples. Journal of Sleep Research, 17, 54–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00627.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kyle, S. D., Sexton, C. E., Feige, B., Luik, A. I., Lane, J., Saxena, R., et al. (2017). Sleep and cognitive performance: Cross-sectional associations in the UK Biobank. Sleep Medicine, 38, 85–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2017.07.001.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Lahl, O., Göritz, A. S., Pietrowsky, R., & Rosenberg, J. (2009). Using the World-Wide Web to obtain large-scale word norms: 190,212 ratings on a set of 2,654 German nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 13–19. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.1.13.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Leong, R. L. F., Cheng, G. H.-L., Chee, M. W. L., & Lo, J. C. (2019). The effects of sleep on prospective memory: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 47, 18–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2019.05.006.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Leong, R. L. F., Koh, S. Y. J., Chee, M. W. L., & Lo, J. C. (2019). Slow wave sleep facilitates spontaneous retrieval in prospective memory. Sleep, 42, zsz003. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsz003.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Leong, R. L. F., Koh, S. Y. J., Tandi, J., Chee, M. W. L., & Lo, J. C. (2018). Multiple nights of partial sleep deprivation do not affect prospective remembering at long delays. Sleep Medicine, 44, 19–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2017.09.037.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Leong, R. L. F., van Rijn, E., Koh, S. Y. J., Chee, M. W. L., & Lo, J. C. (2019). Sleep improves memory for the content but not execution of intentions in adolescents. Sleep Medicine, 56, 111–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2018.12.023.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lim, J., Wu, W.-C., Wang, J., Detre, J. A., Dinges, D. F., & Rao, H. (2010). Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: An ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect. NeuroImage, 49, 3426–3435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.020.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgement of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87, 252–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.252.
Article
Google Scholar
Marsman, M., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2017). Bayesian benefits with JASP. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 545–555. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2016.1259614.
Article
Google Scholar
McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S127–S144. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.775.
Article
Google Scholar
McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2007). Prospective memory: An overview and synthesis of an emerging field. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Google Scholar
Minkel, J., Moreta, M., Muto, J., Htaik, O., Jones, C., Basner, M., & Dinges, D. (2014). Sleep deprivation potentiates HPA axis stress reactivity in healthy adults. Health Psychology, 33, 1430–1434. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034219.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Muehlhan, M., Marxen, M., Landsiedel, J., Malberg, H., & Zaunseder, S. (2014). The effect of body posture on cognitive performance: A question of sleep quality. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 171. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00171.
Occhionero, M., Cicogna, P., & Esposito, M. J. (2017). The effect of sleep loss on dual time-based prospective memory tasks. The American Journal of Psychology, 130, 93–103. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.130.1.0093.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
O'Leary, K., Small, B. J., Panaite, V., Bylsma, L. M., & Rottenberg, J. (2017). Sleep quality in healthy and mood-disordered persons predicts daily life emotional reactivity. Cognition & Emotion, 31, 435–443. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1126554.
Article
Google Scholar
Pavawalla, S. P., Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., & Smith, R. E. (2012). Prospective memory after moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury: A multinomial modeling approach. Neuropsychology, 26, 91–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025866.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Plummer, M. (2013). rjags: Bayesian graphical models using MCMC. R package version, 3.
Google Scholar
Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–163. https://doi.org/10.2307/271063.
Article
Google Scholar
Rendell, P. G., Gray, T. J., Henry, J. D., & Tolan, A. (2007). Prospective memory impairment in “ecstasy” (MDMA) users. Psychopharmacology, 194, 497–504. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-007-0859-z.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rendell, P. G., Mazur, M., & Henry, J. D. (2009). Prospective memory impairment in former users of methamphetamine. Psychopharmacology, 203, 609–616. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1408-0.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Riemann, D., & Backhaus, J. (1996). Behandlung von Schlafstörungen [Treatment of sleep disorders]. Weinheim: Psychologie Verlags Union.
Google Scholar
Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Pratte, M. S. (2017). Bayesian hierarchical models of cognition. In W. H. Batchelder, H. Colonius, E. N. Dzhafarov, & J. Myung (Eds.), New handbook of mathematical psychology (pp. 504–551). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rummel, J., Boywitt, C. D., & Meiser, T. (2011). Assessing the validity of multinomial models using extraneous variables: An application to prospective memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 2194–2210. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.586708.
Article
Google Scholar
Santamaria, J., & Chiappa, K. H. (1987). The EEG of drowsiness in normal adults. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 4, 327–382.
Article
Google Scholar
Schnitzspahn, K. M., Horn, S. S., Bayen, U. J., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Age effects in emotional prospective memory: Cue valence differentially affects the prospective and retrospective component. Psychology and Aging, 27, 498–509. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025021.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Scullin, M. K., Gao, C., Fillmore, P., Roberts, R. L., Pruett, N., & Bliwise, D. L. (2019). Rapid eye movement sleep mediates age-related decline in prospective memory consolidation. Sleep, 42, zsz055. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsz055.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Scullin, M. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2010). Remembering to execute a goal: Sleep on it! Psychological Science, 21, 1028–1035. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610373373.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Scullin, M. K., McDaniel, M. A., & Shelton, J. T. (2013). The Dynamic Multiprocess Framework: Evidence from prospective memory with contextual variability. Cognitive Psychology, 67, 55–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.07.001.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shelton, J. T., Cahill, M. J., Mullet, H. G., Scullin, M. K., Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). Resource depletion does not influence prospective memory in college students. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 1223–1230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.008.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shen, J., Barbera, J., & Shapiro, C. M. (2006). Distinguishing sleepiness and fatigue: Focus on definition and measurement. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10, 63–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.004.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smith, R. E. (2003). The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 347–361. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.347.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smith, R. E., & Bayen, U. J. (2004). A multinomial model of event-based prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 756–777. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.4.756.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smith, R. E., & Bayen, U. J. (2005). The effects of working memory resource availability on prospective memory: A formal modeling approach. Experimental Psychology, 52, 243–256. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.52.4.243.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smith, R. E., & Bayen, U. J. (2006). The source of adult age differences in event-based prospective memory: A multinomial modeling approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 623–635. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.623.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smith, R. E., Bayen, U. J., & Martin, C. (2010). The cognitive processes underlying event-based prospective memory in school-age children and young adults: A formal model-based study. Developmental Psychology, 46, 230–244. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017100.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Smith, R. E., & Hunt, R. R. (2012, April). Prospective Memory: Adult Age, Ongoing Task Difficulty, and Task Importance [Poster presentation]. Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta, GA, United States.
Swann, C. E., Yelland, G. W., Redman, J. R., & Rajaratnam, S. M. W. (2006). Chronic partial sleep loss increases the facilitatory role of a masked prime in a word recognition task. Journal of Sleep Research, 15, 23–29. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00495.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Van Dongen, H. P. A., Maislin, G., Mullington, J. M., & Dinges, D. F. (2003). The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: Dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep, 26, 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/26.2.117.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Van Zandt, T. (2000). ROC curves and confidence judgments in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 582–600. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.26.3.582.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Voigt, C., & Speicher, J. M. (2015). U.S. Patent No. 8,939,500. Washington, DC: U.S. Patent and Trademark Office.
Google Scholar
Waddell, G. (1992). Biopsychosocial analysis of low back pain. Baillière’s Clinical Rheumatology, 6, 523–557. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0950-3579(05)80126-8.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Lee, M., Lodewyckx, T., & Iverson, G. J. (2008). Bayesian versus frequentist inference. In H. Hoijtink, I. Klugkist, & P. A. Boelen (Eds.), Bayesian evaluation of informative hypotheses (pp. 181–207). New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-09612-4_9.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Walter, N. T., & Bayen, U. J. (2016). Selective effects of acute alcohol intake on the prospective and retrospective components of a prospective-memory task with emotional targets. Psychopharmacology, 233, 325–339. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-015-4110-z.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Young, T. B. (2004). Epidemiology of daytime sleepiness: Definitions, symptomatology, and prevalence. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 65, S12–S16.
Google Scholar