Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 179–211.
Article
Google Scholar
Arkes, H. R., Christensen, C., Lai, C., & Blumer, C. (1987). Two methods of reducing overconfidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39(1), 133–144.
Article
Google Scholar
Atkins, L., & Michie, S. (2013). Changing eating behaviour: what can we learn from behavioural science? Nutrition Bulletin, 38(1), 30–35.
Article
Google Scholar
Barber, B. M., & Odean, T. (2001). Boys will be boys: gender, overconfidence, and common stock investment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, 261–292.
Article
Google Scholar
Batchelder, S., Rizzo, M., Vanderleest, R., & Vecera, S. (2003). Traffic Scene Related Change Blindness in Older Drivers. In: Proceedings of the Second International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, July 21-24, 2003, Park City, Utah. Iowa City: Public Policy Center, of Iowa, 2003: 177-181. https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1117.
Bengtsson, C., Persson, M., & Willenhag, P. (2005). Gender and overconfidence. Economics Letters, 86(2), 199–203.
Article
Google Scholar
Bonneh, Y. S., Cooperman, A., & Sagi, D. (2001). Motion-induced blindness in normal observers. Nature, 411(6839), 798–801.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Boyatzis, R. E. (1998). Transforming qualitative information: thematic analysis and code development. Thousand Oaks, London, & New Delhi: SAGE Publications.
Google Scholar
Clayson, D. E. (2005). Performance overconfidence: metacognitive effects or misplaced student expectations? Journal of Marketing Education, 27(2), 122–129.
Article
Google Scholar
Colman, A. M., Norris, C. E., & Preston, C. C. (1997). Comparing rating scales of different lengths: equivalence of scores from 5-point and 7-point scales. Psychological Reports, 80, 355–362.
Article
Google Scholar
Dawes, J. G. (2008). Do data characteristics change according to the number of scale points used? An experiment using 5 point, 7 point and 10 point scales. International Journal of Market Research, 51(1), 61–77.
Article
Google Scholar
De Pelsmacker, P., & Janssens, W. (2007). The effect of norms, attitudes and habits on speeding behavior: scale development and model building and estimation. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 39(1), 6–15.
Article
Google Scholar
Deery, H. A. (2000). Hazard and risk perception among young novice drivers. Journal of Safety Research, 30(4), 225–236.
Article
Google Scholar
Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Finn, P., & Bragg, B. W. (1986). Perception of the risk of an accident by young and older drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 18(4), 289–298.
Article
Google Scholar
Ghiselli, E. E. (1955). The measurement of occupational aptitude. Berkeley: Univers. of California.
Google Scholar
Grimes, J. (1996). On the failure to detect changes in scenes across saccades. In K. Akins (Ed.), Perception, (vol. 2). New York: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Hammond, D., McDonald, P. W., Fong, G. T., Brown, K. S., & Cameron, R. (2004). The impact of cigarette warning labels and smoke-free bylaws on smoking cessation: evidence from former smokers. Canadian Journal of Public Health/Revue Canadienne de Sante’e Publique, 95, 201–204.
Google Scholar
Harré, N., Foster, S., & O'Neill, M. (2005). Self-enhancement, crash-risk optimism and the impact of safety advertisements on young drivers. British Journal of Psychology, 96(2), 215–230.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hedlund, J. (2000). Risky business: safety regulations, risk compensation, and individual behavior. Injury Prevention, 6(2), 82–89.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The role of fixation position in detecting scene changes across saccades. Psychological Science, 10(5), 438–443.
Article
Google Scholar
Hoekstra, T., & Wegman, F. (2011). Improving the effectiveness of road safety campaigns: current and new practices. IATSS Research, 34(2), 80–86.
Article
Google Scholar
Jackson, C., Eliasson, L., Barber, N., & Weinman, J. (2014). Applying COM-B to medication adherence. The European Health Psychologist, 16, 7–17.
Google Scholar
Katila, A., Keskinen, E., & Hatakka, M. (1996). Contlicting goals of skid training. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 28(6), 785–789.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Katila, A., Keskinen, E., Hatakka, M., & Laapotti, S. (2004). Does increased confidence among novice drivers imply a decrease in safety?: the effects of skid training on slippery road accidents. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 36(4), 543–550.
Article
Google Scholar
Kunar, M. A., Carter, R., Cohen, M., & Horowitz, T. (2008). Telephone conversation impairs sustained visual attention via a central Bottleneck. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1135–1140.
Article
Google Scholar
Kunar, M. A., Cole, L., Cox, A., & Ocampo, J. (2018). It is not good to talk: conversation has a fixed interference cost on attention regardless of difficulty. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3, 33.
Google Scholar
Kunar, M. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2006). Object-based inhibitory priming in preview search: evidence from the “top-up” procedure. Memory & Cognition, 34, 459–474.
Article
Google Scholar
Kunar, M. A., Rich, A. N., & Wolfe, J. M. (2010). Spatial and temporal separation fails to counteract the effects of low prevalence in visual search. Visual Cognition, 18, 881–897.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kunar, M. A., & Watson, D. G. (2011). Visual search in a multi-element asynchronous dynamic (MAD) world. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(4), 1017–1031.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kunar, M. A., & Watson, D. G. (2014). When are abrupt onsets found efficiently in complex visual search?: evidence from multi-element asynchronous dynamic search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 232–252.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kunar, M. A., Watson, D. G., Taylor-Phillips, S., & Wolska, J. (2017). Low prevalence search for cancers in mammograms: evidence using laboratory experiments and computer aided detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 23, 369–385.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lajunen, T., & Summala, H. (2003). Can we trust self-reports of driving? Effects of impression management on driver behaviour questionnaire responses. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 6(2), 97–107.
Article
Google Scholar
Lees, M. N., Sparks, J., Lee, J. D., & Rizzo, M. (2007). Change Blindness, Attention, and Driving Performance. In: Proceedings of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, July 9-12, 2007, Stevenson, Washington. Iowa City: Public Policy Center, University of Iowa, 2007: 32-38. https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1211.
Levin, D. T., Momen, N., Drivdahl IV, S. B., & Simons, D. J. (2000). Change blindness blindness: the metacognitive error of overestimating change-detection ability. Visual Cognition, 7(1–3), 397–412.
Article
Google Scholar
Lundeberg, M. A., Fox, P. W., & Punćcohaŕ, J. (1994). Highly confident but wrong: gender differences and similarities in confidence judgments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 114–121.
Article
Google Scholar
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2006). Culture and change blindness. Cognitive Science, 30, 381–399.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
McCarley, J. S., Vais, M. J., Pringle, H., Kramer, A. F., Irwin, D. E., & Strayer, D. L. (2004). Conversation disrupts change detection in complex traffic scenes. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 46(3), 424–436.
Article
Google Scholar
Menkhoff, L., Schmeling, M., & Schmidt, U. (2013). Overconfidence, experience, and professionalism: an experimental study. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 86, 92–101.
Article
Google Scholar
Michie, S., van Stralen, M. M., & West, R. (2011). The behaviour change wheel: a new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implementation Science, 6(1), 42.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Moore, D. A., & Healy, P. J. (2008). The trouble with overconfidence. Psychological Review, 115(2), 502–517.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mynttinen, S., Sundström, A., Koivukoski, M., Hakuli, K., Keskinen, E., & Henriksson, W. (2009). Are novice drivers overconfident? A comparison of self-assessed and examiner-assessed driver competences in a Finnish and Swedish sample. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 12(2), 120–130.
Article
Google Scholar
Nederhof, A. J. (1985). Methods of coping with social desirability bias: a review. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15(3), 263–280.
Article
Google Scholar
Nemme, H. E., & White, K. M. (2010). Texting while driving: psychosocial influences on young people’s texting intentions and behaviour. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(4), 1257–1265.
Article
Google Scholar
Niederle, M., & Vesterlund, L. (2007). Do women shy away from competition? Do men compete too much? The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 1067–1101.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K., Deubel, H., Clark, J. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2000). Picture changes during blinks: looking without seeing and seeing without looking. Visual Cognition, 7, 191–211.
Article
Google Scholar
Peterson, S., Hoffer, G., & Millner, E. (1995). Are drivers of air-bag-equipped cars more aggressive? A test of the offsetting hypothesis. Journal of Law and Economics, 38, 251–264.
Article
Google Scholar
Pliske, R. M., & Mutter, S. A. (1996). Age differences in the accuracy of confidence judgments. Experimental Aging Research, 22(2), 199–216.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pulford, B. D., & Colman, A. M. (1997). Overconfidence: feedback and item difficulty effects. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(1), 125–133.
Article
Google Scholar
Reason, J., Manstead, A., Stradling, S., Baxter, J., & Campbell, K. (1990). Errors and violations on the roads: a real distinction? Ergonomics, 33(10–11), 1315–1332.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368–373.
Article
Google Scholar
Rosenbloom, T., Shahar, A., Elharar, A., & Danino, O. (2008). Risk perception of driving as a function of advanced training aimed at recognizing and handling risks in demanding driving situations. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 40(2), 697–703.
Article
Google Scholar
Ruiter, R. A., Abraham, C., & Kok, G. (2001). Scary warnings and rational precautions: a review of the psychology of fear appeals. Psychology and Health, 16(6), 613–630.
Article
Google Scholar
Russell, N., & Kunar, M. A. (2012). Color and spatial cueing in low prevalence visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1327–1344.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sandroni, A., & Squintani, F. (2004). A survey on overconfidence, insurance, and self-assessment training programs. Working paper, Kellogg School of Management.
Shinar, D., Schechtman, E., & Compton, R. (2001). Self-reports of safe driving behaviors in relationship to sex, age, education and income in the US adult driving population. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 33(1), 111–116.
Article
Google Scholar
Simons, D. J., Franconeri, S. L., & Reimer, R. L. (2000). Change blindness in the absence of a visual disruption. Perception-London, 29(10), 1143–1154.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(4), 644–649.
Article
Google Scholar
Statman, M., Thorley, S., & Vorkink, K. (2006). Investor overconfidence and trading volume. Review of Financial Studies, 19(4), 1531–1565.
Article
Google Scholar
Stevenson, M. R., Palamara, P., Morrison, D., & Ryan, G. A. (2001). Behavioral factors as predictors of motor vehicle crashes in young drivers. Crash Prevention and Injury Control, 2(4), 247–254.
Article
Google Scholar
Stirk, J. A., & Underwood, G. (2007). Low-level visual saliency does not predict change detection in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 7(10), 3.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction: dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Science, 12(6), 462–466.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Svenson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta Psychologica, 47(2), 143–148.
Article
Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97–136.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Van Wert, M. J., Horowitz, T. S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2009). Even in correctable search, some types of rare targets are frequently missed. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71(3), 541–553.
Article
Google Scholar
Vrolix, K. (2006). Behavioural adaptation, risk compensation, risk homeostasis and moral hazard in traffic safety, (pp. 1–59). Universiteit Hasselt Literature, Review. RA-2006-95 Retrieved from: https://doclib.uhasselt.be/dspace/bitstream/1942/4002/1/behavioraladaptation.pdf. Accessed 29 Apr 2019.
Watson, D. G., & Kunar, M. A. (2010). Visual marking and change blindness: moving occluders and transient masks neutralize shape changes to ignored objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1391–1405.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Weenig, M. W., & Midden, C. J. (1997). Mass-media information campaigns and knowledge-gap effects. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27(11), 945–958.
Article
Google Scholar
Wilde, G. J. S. (1982). The theory of risk homeostasis: implications for safety and health. Risk Analysis, 2, 209–225.
Article
Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Birnkrant, R. S., Kunar, M. A., & Horowitz, T. (2005). Visual search for transparency and opacity: attentional guidance by cue combination? Journal of Vision, 5, 257–274.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., & Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual-search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 419–433.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Horowitz, T. S., & Kenner, N. M. (2005). Rare items often missed in visual search. Nature, 435, 439–440.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Horowitz, T. S., Ven Wert, M. J., Kenner, N. M., Place, S. S., & Kibbi, N. (2007). Low target prevalence is a stubborn source of errors in visual search tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 136(4), 623–638.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar