Andre, J., & Rogers, S. (2006). Using verbal and blind-walking distance estimates to investigate the two visual systems hypothesis. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(3), 353–361.
Article
Google Scholar
Bach, D. R., Furl, N., Barnes, G., & Dolan, R. J. (2015). Sustained magnetic responses in temporal cortex reflect instantaneous significance of approaching and receding sounds. PLoS One, 10(7), e0134060.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bach, D. R., Neuhoff, J. G., Perrig, W., & Seifritz, E. (2009). Looming sounds as warning signals: The function of motion cues. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74(1), 28–33. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.06.004
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bach, D. R., Schachinger, H., Neuhoff, J. G., Esposito, F., Di Salle, F., Lehmann, C., . . . Seifritz, E. (2008). Rising sound intensity: An intrinsic warning cue activating the amygdala. Cerebral Cortex, 18(1), 145–150. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm040
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5. doi:10.1177/1745691610393980
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Canevet, G., Teghtsoonian, R., & Teghtsoonian, M. (2003). A comparison of loudness change in signals that continuously rise or fall in amplitude. Acta Acustica United with Acustica, 89(2), 339–345.
Google Scholar
Cappe, C., Thelen, A., Romei, V., Thut, G., & Murray, M. M. (2012). Looming signals reveal synergistic principles of multisensory integration. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(4), 1171–1182. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5517-11.2012
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Carlile, S., & Best, V. (2002). Discrimination of sound source velocity in human listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111(2), 1026–1035. doi:10.1121/1.1436067
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Carlile, S., & Leung, J. (2016). The perception of auditory motion. Trends in Hearing, 20, 2331216516644254. doi:10.1177/2331216516614254
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Cauchoix, M., & Chaine, A. S. (2016). How can we study the evolution of animal minds? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 18. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00358
Article
Google Scholar
Croston, R., Branch, C. L., Kozlovsky, D. Y., Dukas, R., & Pravosudov, V. V. (2015). Heritability and the evolution of cognitive traits. Behavioral Ecology, 26(6), 1447–1459. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv088
Article
Google Scholar
Crump, M. J. C., McDonnell, J. V., & Gureckis, T. M. (2013). Evaluating Amazon’s mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral research. PLoS One, 8(3), e57410. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057410
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Edwards, M., & Ibbotson, M. R. (2007). Relative sensitivities to large-field optic-flow patterns varying in direction and speed. Perception, 36(1), 113–124. doi:10.1068/p5626
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fletcher, P. D., Nicholas, J. M., Shakespeare, T. J., Downey, L. E., Golden, H. L., Agustus, J. L., . . . Warren, J. D. (2015). Dementias show differential physiological responses to salient sounds. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 73. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00073
Freeman, T. C. A., Leung, J., Wufong, E., Orchard-Mills, E., Carlile, S., & Alais, D. (2014). Discrimination contours for moving sounds reveal duration and distance cues dominate auditory speed perception. PLoS One, 9(7), e102864. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102864
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Freiberg, K., Tually, K., & Crassini, B. (2001). Use of an auditory looming task to test infants’ sensitivity to sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 1–10. doi:10.1348/026151001165903
Article
Google Scholar
Gardner, W. G., & Martin, K. D. (1995). HRTF measurements of a KEMAR. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(6), 3907–3908. doi:10.1121/1.412407
Article
Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., & Maier, J. X. (2009). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) hear rising frequency sounds as looming. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123(4), 822–827. doi:10.1037/a0016391
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., Neuhoff, J. G., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Auditory looming perception in rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(24), 15755–15757. doi:10.1073/pnas.242469699
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Glatz, C., Bulthoff, H. H., & Chuang, L. L. (2014). Looming auditory warnings initiate earlier event-related potentials in a manual steering task. Cognitive Processing, 15(1), S38–S38.
Google Scholar
Gordon, M. S., & Rosenblum, L. D. (2005). Effects of intrastimulus modality change on audiovisual time-to-arrival judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(4), 580–594. doi:10.3758/bf03193516
Article
Google Scholar
Grantham, D. W. (1997). Auditory motion perception: Snapshots revisited. In R. H. Gilkey & T. R. Anderson (Eds.), Binaural and spatial hearing in real and virtual environments. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Google Scholar
Grassi, M. (2010). Sex difference in subjective duration of looming and receding sounds. Perception, 39(10), 1424–1426. doi:10.1068/p6810
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Guski, R. (1992). Acoustic tau: An easy analogue to visual tau? Ecological Psychology, 4, 189–197.
Article
Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G., Bryant, G. A., Wilke, A., Frederick, D. A., Galperin, A., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Moore, T. (2009). Adaptive rationality: an evolutionary perspective on cognitive bias. Social Cognition, 27(5), 733–763.
Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 81–91. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.81
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G., & Nettle, D. (2006). The paranoid optimist: An integrative evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 47–66. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_3
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Holden, C. J., Dennie, T., & Hicks, A. D. (2013). Assessing the reliability of the M5-120 on Amazon’s mechanical Turk. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1749–1754. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.020
Article
Google Scholar
Ingham, N. J., Hart, H. C., & McAlpine, D. (2001). Spatial receptive fields of inferior colliculus neurons to auditory apparent motion in free field. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85(1), 23–33.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lewis, C. F., & McBeath, M. K. (2004). Bias to experience approaching motion in a three-dimensional virtual environment. Perception, 33(3), 259–276. doi:10.1068/p5190
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Locke, S. M., Leung, J., & Carlile, S. (2016). Sensitivity to auditory velocity contrast. Scientific Reports, 6, 5. doi:10.1038/srep27725
Article
Google Scholar
Maier, J. X., Chandrasekaran, C., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2008). Integration of bimodal looming signals through neuronal coherence in the temporal lobe. Current Biology, 18(13), 963–968. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.05.043
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Maier, J. X., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2007). Looming biases in monkey auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(15), 4093–4100. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0330-07.2007
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Maier, J. X., Neuhoff, J. G., Logothetis, N. K., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2004). Multisensory integration of looming signals by Rhesus monkeys. Neuron, 43(2), 177–181. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.06.027
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
McBeath, M. K., Shaffer, D. M., & Kaiser, M. K. (1995). How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. Science, 268(5210), 569–573. doi:10.1126/science.7725104
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
McGuire, A. B., Gillath, O., & Vitevitch, M. (2016). Effects of mental resource availability on looming task performance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 107–113.
Article
Google Scholar
Morrongiello, B. A., Hewitt, K. L., & Gotowiec, A. (1991). Infants discrimination of relative distance in the auditory modality - approaching versus receding sound sources. Infant Behavior & Development, 14(2), 187–208. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(91)90005-d
Article
Google Scholar
Mueller, A. S., & Timney, B. (2014). Effects of radial direction and eccentricity on acceleration perception. Perception, 43(8), 805–810. doi:10.1068/p7776
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2015). Traffic Safety Facts 2013 Data - Pedestrians. Retrieved from Washington, DC: http://www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/Pubs/812124.pdf
Neuhoff, J. G. (1998). Perceptual bias for rising tones. Nature, 395(6698), 123–124. doi:10.1038/25862
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Neuhoff, J. (1999). Perception of changes in loudness - Reply. Nature, 398(6729), 673–674. doi:10.1038/19446
Article
Google Scholar
Neuhoff, J. G. (2001). An adaptive bias in the perception of looming auditory motion. Ecological Psychology, 13, 87–110.
Article
Google Scholar
Neuhoff, J. G., Hamilton, G. R., Gittleson, A. L., & Mejia, A. (2014). Babies in traffic: Infant vocalizations and listener sex modulate auditory motion perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 40(2), 775–783. doi:10.1037/a0035071
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Neuhoff, J. G., Long, K. L., & Worthington, R. C. (2012). Strength and physical fitness predict the perception of looming sounds. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(4), 318–322. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.11.001
Article
Google Scholar
Neuhoff, J. G., Planisek, R., & Seifritz, E. (2009). Adaptive sex differences in auditory motion perception: Looming sounds are special. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 35(1), 225–234. doi:10.1037/a0013159
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(5), 411–419.
Google Scholar
Recarte, M. A., Conchillo, A., & Nunes, L. M. (2000). Perception of Speed and Increments in Cars. Berne, Switzerland: Paper presented at the International Conference on Traffic and Transport Psychology.
Google Scholar
Recarte, M. A., & Nunes, L. M. (1996). Perception of speed in automobile: Estimation and production. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied, 2(4), 291–304.
Article
Google Scholar
Regan, D., & Vincent, A. (1995). Visual processing of looming and time to contact throughout the visual-field. Vision Research, 35(13), 1845–1857. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)00274-p
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Riskind, J. H., Kleiman, E. M., Seifritz, E., & Neuhoff, J. G. (2014). Influence of anxiety, depression and looming cognitive style on auditory looming perception. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(1), 45–50. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.11.005
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rosenblum, L. D. (2008). Speech perception as a multimodal phenomenon. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(6), 405–409. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00615.x
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Rosenblum, L. D., Carello, C., & Pastore, R. E. (1987). Relative effectiveness of 3 stimulus variables for locating a moving sound source. Perception, 16(2), 175–186. doi:10.1068/p160175
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rosenblum, L. D., Gordon, M. S., & Wuestefeld, A. P. (2000). Effects of performance feedback and feedback withdrawal on auditory looming perception. Ecological Psychology, 12(4), 273–291. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco1204_02
Article
Google Scholar
Rosenblum, L. D., Wuestefeld, A. P., & Saldana, H. M. (1993). Auditory looming perception - influences on anticipatory judgments. Perception, 22(12), 1467–1482. doi:10.1068/p221467
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Schiff, W., & Oldak, R. (1990). Accuracy of judging time to arrival - effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 16, 2. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.2.303
Article
Google Scholar
Scott, T. R., Lavender, A. D., McWhirt, R. A., & Powell, D. A. (1966). Directional asymmetry of motion aftereffect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(6), 806. doi:10.1037/h0023194
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Seifritz, E., Neuhoff, J. G., Bilecen, D., Scheffler, K., Mustovic, H., Schachinger, H., . . . Di Salle, F. (2002). Neural processing of auditory looming in the human brain. Current Biology, 12, 24. doi:10.1016/s0960-9822(02)01356-8
Senna, I., Parise, C. V., & Ernst, M. O. (2015). Hearing in slow-motion: Humans underestimate the speed of moving sounds. Scientific Reports, 5, 14054. doi:10.1038/srep14054
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Tajadura-Jimenez, A., Valjamae, A., Asutay, E., & Vastfjall, D. (2010). Embodied auditory perception: The emotional impact of approaching and receding sound sources. Emotion, 10(2), 216–229. doi:10.1037/a0018422
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Teghtsoonian, R., Teghtsoonian, M., & Canevet, G. (2005). Sweep-induced acceleration in loudness change and the "bias for rising intensities". Perception & Psychophysics, 67(4), 699–712. doi:10.3758/bf03193526
Article
Google Scholar
Triggs, T. J., & Berenyi, J. S. (1982). Estimation of automobile speed under day and night conditions. Human Factors, 24(1), 111–114.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Warren, R. M. (1981). Measurement of sensory intensity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4(2), 175–189.
Article
Google Scholar
Warren, J. D., Zielinski, B. A., Green, G. G. R., Rauschecker, J. P., & Griffiths, T. D. (2002). Perception of sound-source motion by the human brain. Neuron, 34(1), 139–148. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00637-2
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wilson, W. W., & O’Neill, W. E. (1998). Auditory motion induces directionally dependent receptive field shifts in inferior colliculus neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(4), 2040–2062.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Witten, I. B., Bergan, J. F., & Knudsen, E. I. (2006). Dynamic shifts in the owl’s auditory space map predict moving sound location. Nature Neuroscience, 9(11), 1439–1445. doi:10.1038/nn1781
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar