Skip to main content

Face coverings: Considering the implications for face perception and speech communication

A Correction to this article was published on 30 May 2023

This article has been updated

Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications has released a comprehensive collection of thirty articles on the impact of face coverings for face perception and speech communication. This thematic series was motivated by the global COVID-19 pandemic in 2020, when many governments around the world required or strongly recommended the wearing of face coverings (masks) in public spaces. It seems likely that the use of face coverings will continue, albeit in a more selective manner, even now that the acute phase of the COVID-19 pandemic appears to be over.

Research on the impacts of face coverings on communication increased massively in 2020, with early work establishing, unsurprisingly, that masks hinder the recognition of identity and expression. Masks also impair speech communication and understanding and as a consequence can negatively affect psychosocial well-being. Papers included in this thematic collection represent the second phase of research, where more nuanced effects are investigated and the processes underpinning the effects found are explored. Papers included in this thematic series can be grouped by topic.

A number of papers explored the role of mask wearing in identity recognition, learning and matching. Previous research established that wearing a mask impairs recognition (see for example, Carragher & Hancock, 2020; Freud et al., 2020), however the extent and range of this effect and its mechanisms are less well understood. To this aim, in this thematic series, the detrimental effects of masks were investigated in children (Stajduhar & Freud, 2022) and in adults with Autistic Spectrum Disorder (Tso et al., 2022). Despite the detrimental effect of masks on famous face identification (Wong & Estudillo, 2022) familiarity-detection could still occur (Carlow et al., 2022). Research showed that it was harder to remember not only the identity of unfamiliar faces but whether or not that unfamiliar face wore a mask (Kollenda & de Haas, 2022).

For face identity matching, Bennetts et al. (2022) found decreased performance with masks and sunglasses but no impact of face mask exposure. Interestingly, although Estudillo and Wong (2022) found that face masks disrupted matching performance, for ‘match’ trials there was better performance when both faces were wearing masks rather than just one. Similarly, in a forensic context, identification of a masked perpetrator was increased when a masked line-up was also used (target present condition; Manley et al., 2022). The importance of congruity between stimuli is highlighted. This is consistent with the paper by Thorley et al. (2022) who found that eyewitnesses may struggle when perpetrators wear face masks during offences (but not at identification). They also found inaccurate age estimates of people who wear face masks (also see Ganel & Goodale, 2022; Wong & Estudillo, 2022). The paper by Carragher et al., (2022) suggests that some of the deficit in masked face matching may be alleviated by feature-based training.

The relationship between stimuli at face learning and recognition was explored by Hsaio et al. (2022) using an eye-tracking methodology. Results showed that eye movements during recognition were mainly driven by the mask condition during recognition but not that during learning and those who adjusted their strategy according to the mask condition difference between learning and recognition had better performance. Further work has explored the dynamic interplay between mask wearing at encoding and recognition (Garcia-Marques et al., 2022).

In a second group, several papers examined the effect of wearing a face mask on the ability to recognise emotion from facial expressions. Previous research has shown that face masks impair the ability to perceive social information and the recognition of emotion (see review by Pavlova & Sokolov, 2022). Whilst research shows that different mask designs had equivalent detrimental effects on recognition accuracy (Blazenkova et al., 2022), some people may be more negatively impacted by masks than others (Swain et al., 2022). Interestingly, some of the negative effects of face masks on the understanding of emotional states were reduced by the use of transparent masks (McCrackin et al., 2022).

Grenville and Dwyer (2022) found that there was an overall emotion recognition accuracy for faces shown without masks compared to when shown wearing them, but this effect varied by emotion (advantage without masks in disgust, happiness, and sadness; no effect for neutral; lower accuracy without masks for anger and fear). Using a similar static-based face emotion recognition task, Rinke et al. (2022) showed that the impairment was largest for disgust, followed by fear, surprise, sadness, and happiness. It was not significant for anger (also see Wong & Estudillo, 2022) or neutral expressions. Here, they concluded that participants were likely to confuse emotions that share activation of the visible muscles in the upper half of the face. Using dynamic face stimuli, Henke et al. (2022) also found that face masks reduced emotion recognition accuracy and confidence in both younger and older participants. A few papers looked at other aspects of faces including gender decisions (Wong & Estudillo, 2022) and face attractiveness (Hies & Lewis, 2022; Pazhoohi et al., 2022).

The final set of papers looked at ways in which face masks affected communication in terms of understanding, interactions and interpretation. In a large survey conducted in Australia, Galvin et al., (2022) found that face masks negatively affected the quality of communication, feelings about communication, and led to increased fatigue and frustration and decreased time spent communicating. Lee et al. (2022) found similar results for communication in healthcare settings, emphasizing the added cognitive load experienced by both patients and providers, and associated decreased clinical efficiency. Crinnion et al., (2022) showed problems with masked speech recognition did not change over the course of a year. Sinagra and Wiener (2022) noted that masks can make it more difficult to understand the intended intonation and emotional meaning of speech, while Gutierriz-Sigut et al. (2022); Lee et al. (2022) and Lau et al. (2022) all noted that the negative impacts of face masks were greater for people who are deaf or hard of hearing. At a societal level, the data of Krishna et al., (2021) collected via an online approach-avoidance task indicated that attitudes towards masks and COVID-19 anxiety influenced attitudes towards mask wearers.

A number of studies investigated strategies and interventions to manage the impacts of mask wearing. Poon and Jenstad (2022) identified several practical ways to support people who are D/deaf or hard of hearing. These included using transparent masks, improved guidance on when to wear masks, and educating the public about ways to communicate clearly when wearing a mask. Finally, Gutz et al., (2022) showed that while speaking loudly or clearly can compensate for the presence of a mask, such strategies require an increased physical and cognitive effort by the speaker.

This thematic series has raised many new questions about how face perception and communication is influenced by face coverings. For example, we must extend research to include the realistic scenario where masks are worn on dynamic faces. Here the way the face moves behind the mask may give clues about both the identity of the person shown and the expression they are displaying. Individual characteristic movements are known to aid familiar face recognition (Lander et al., 1999) and such parameters may also be present from a face wearing a mask. Similarly, other individual differences in the wearer and mask (for example, size of face, relative size and location of face mask, way face mask is worn etc.); and the perceiver (for example, face recognition ability etc.) may be important in determining the specific impact of face masks in different scenarios. We must consider in more detail why face masks impair performance. It may be that masks impair accuracy as they reduce the amount of local face information available to observers. Alternatively (or additionally), negative effects of face masks may arise from the disruption of normal holistic processing of faces (Tanaka & Farah, 1993). Thus, face mask research may also tell us more generally about face perception and recognition.

Further the effects of face masks on communication go beyond the direct effects associated with the attenuation of the acoustic signal, to the psychosocial and psychological realm. This especially true for people with hearing loss. It remains to be seen whether a transparent mask can be developed that is both visually and acoustically transparent which would remove the communication barriers that masks currently impose. We look forward to seeing the next wave of research considering the implications of face coverings for face perception and speech communication, topics that have both theoretical and applied interest.

Change history

References

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karen Lander.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lander, K., Saunders, G.H. Face coverings: Considering the implications for face perception and speech communication. Cogn. Research 8, 24 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-023-00479-w

Download citation

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-023-00479-w